Literature summary with the prescribed articles for The Adolescent Brain (LU) 21/22

Literature summary with the prescribed articles for The Adolescent Brain (LU) 21/22

Image

Check summaries and supporting content in full:
Article summary of The social re-orientation of adolescence by Nelson et al. - 2005 - Chapter

Article summary of The social re-orientation of adolescence by Nelson et al. - 2005 - Chapter

Introduction

The social brain undergoes many changes during adolescence. Research shows that changes in social behaviour are caused by sociocultural factors and innate characteristics. Both ensure that the brain adjusts naturally on a social level. In addition, abnormal emotional responses to social situations are also important for mood and anxiety disorders during adolescence.

Social information processing

Social information processing requires complex neuronal systems. One must quickly identify a particular stimulus as being 'social' and then integrate this stimulus into a larger, already existing, emotional and cognitive network. This requires communication between brain areas that deal with social detection and brain areas that process affective and cognitive information.

The following model is discussed in this article: Social Information Processing Network (SIPN). This model consists of three key points:

  1. Detection. Here it is determined whether or not a stimulus can be seen as 'social'. Various brain structures are involved, such as the inferior occipital cortex, the intraparietal sulcus and the fusiform face area. The various brain areas involved include perceptual functions.

  2. Affection. These are areas of the brain that deal with reward and punishment, including the amygdala, the septum, and the hypothalamus. For example, this determines whether the social stimulus should be approached or avoided. In addition, autonomous and cognitive processes are arranged here in order to be able to respond adequately to the social stimulus.

  3. Cognitive regulation. This involves three different processes:

  • Processing of the mental state of the other (Theory of Mind) by the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex.

  • Inhibition of violent reactions by the ventral prefrontal cortex.

  • Facilitate goal-oriented behaviour by the dorsal and ventral prefrontal cortex. It is important to achieve goals to control emotional behaviour. Motivation to achieve a goal also plays a role here.

The ventral cortex plays a role in both affection and cognitive regulation.

The SIPN model assumes that the aforementioned processes take place one after the other. However, when looking at the brain, one can see that the three processes are part of an interactive network. For example, the detection areas receive information from the affection areas to be able to interpret a stimulus as social.

Changes in the SIPN model in adolescence

There are many changes in social behaviour during adolescence. Three major changes are sexuality, being more focused on peers and less focused on parents.

  • Detection. The associated brain structures mature early in life. Thus, there are no known changes to these brain structures during adolescence since the structures are already mature.

  • Affection. During puberty, there are both functional and anatomical changes in the corresponding brain structures. This is caused by sex steroids (i.e. sex hormones). This steroid regulates other neurotransmitter systems, for example, the systems of dopamine, serotonin and oxytocin, all involved in social behaviour. Sex steroids are involved in behavioural changes in social responses. The most striking is the sexual reactivity. Some studies have also found that steroids play a role in dominant behaviour and conflicts with parents during puberty. Other studies find links between sex hormones and sexual behaviour, parental behaviour, social bonding and social memory. Functional changes in the areas of the brain related to affection have an effect on the amount of sex hormone that is released. There are also studies that show the link between hormones and new stimuli, which makes the adolescence period very important for developing patterns of social behaviour.

  • Cognitive regulation. People with damage to the prefrontal cortex have problems with social awareness and making social decisions. The earlier in development these areas are damaged, the more intense the effects are. Parts of the prefrontal cortex have not matured until the end of the teenage years or early twenties, which means that for example, inhibition control is very difficult for younger people. More myelin and the pruning of existing networks leads to changes in the field of cognitive regulation. These changes are slower than the changes in the affective areas. The hormones also play no role in this. Interaction with the affective areas can, however, ensure that hormone levels play a secondary role in the changes in the brain involved in cognitive regulation.

Neuroimaging techniques and changes in the SIPN model

The central nervous system mediates changes in the social brain areas during adolescence. Very little is known about which changes are taking place within the areas as discussed in the SIPN model. A recent study shows that different activation patterns are visible in the brain in response to social stimuli when comparing young adults with adolescents. We looked at the areas associated with affection and cognitive regulation. The activation in the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate is higher in adolescents when they see frightened faces than in young adults.

When we look at important changes at the behavioural level during adolescence, contact with peers should provide more and more reward as the adolescent gets older. When contacting the parents this is exactly the other way around. In the brain, this should be reflected in increased activation in the brain areas associated with affection, when stimuli from peers are offered. In addition, these stimuli should have a stronger effect on attention and memory processes. The same should also happen with potential sexual partners. These assumptions can be investigated with fMRI.

Studies show that adolescents are very sensitive to peer acceptance and rejection. Neuroimaging techniques could also be used for this. In the areas of the brain related to affection, activation should be seen in terms of motivation, self-confidence, and social acceptance and rejection.

Although the Theory of Mind is a well-known phenomenon, very little research has been done about the changes during adolescence. This is probably because the Theory of Mind originates in early childhood. Recent research implies that regions in the dorsomedial cortex play an important role in the Theory of Mind. Changes take place in this area until early adulthood. fMRI tasks in which one subject (thinks he) interacts with another can play a role in understanding the changes in Theory of Mind during adolescence.

The brain development according to the SIPN model and affective disorders

During adolescence, the number of people with an affective or specific anxiety disorder increases considerably. Changes in the processing of social stimuli as stated in the SIPN model seem important here. In adolescence, people seem to be very responsive to social stimuli and social events. Changes in emotional motivation play a role here. For some adolescents, these emotions are so strong that there is psychopathology.

Rejection by a possible romantic partner or by peers also plays an important role in the development of psychopathology during adolescence. Difficulties regarding social relationships are also related to suicide risk. During early adolescence, suicide risk is more closely related to the relationship with parents, while suicide risk is more closely related to romantic relationships during late adolescence. This suggests changes in the brain areas related to affect.

Especially in women, there is a large correlation between stress and emotion during adolescence.

The extra stress during adolescence contributes to psychopathology. We know that a good social network can serve as a buffer for stress-related events. In adolescents, however, the social network is undergoing a change (fewer parents and direct family, more peers), while the amount of stress (for example due to academic achievement) increases. Social integration in a renewed social network is largely dependent on the brain areas involved in the SIPN model.

During the adolescence period, there appears to be a 'mismatch' between the affective systems and the systems involved in cognitive regulation. This is because the affective systems mature quickly, while the cognitive regulation systems lag behind, due to their slow development. As a result, some adolescents experience intense emotional reactions, while their inhibitory capacity, for example, cannot yet mediate.

During adolescence, women suffer from mood and anxiety disorders twice as often as men. Gender differences in social behaviour are present throughout life, but reach a peak during adolescence. The reason that women are more often confronted with psychopathology is probably the fact that women rely more on social support and suffer more from rejection than men. Recent research, however, shows that there are more complicated mechanisms behind it, and the type of stressor, among other things, needs to be considered. The areas as discussed in the SIPN model are more sensitive to stress in women than in men.

Recent research seems to indicate abnormalities in structures that are part of the SIPN model when there is an anxiety or mood disorder. This relates to the superior temporal gyrus, the ventral prefrontal cortex and the amygdala. The level of white matter in the frontal areas and the levels of choline in the orbitofrontal cortex also play a role.

New fMRI tasks are desperately needed to expose psychopathology in the brain during adolescence. For example, one could look at social rejection, where a very strong negative reaction should occur in women with mood or anxiety disorders.

Conclusion

The social behaviour of adolescents undergoes many changes. This is caused by neuronal changes, caused by hormones, maturation and learning processes. It is important to expose these changes with neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI. Among other things, this helps us to understand the increased risk of psychopathology in adolescence. There seems to be a strong relationship between psychopathology in adolescence and the SIPN model.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior by Casey - 2015 - Chapter

Article summary of Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior by Casey - 2015 - Chapter

Introduction

Adolescence can be seen as a strong contrasting period. In this period people start looking for sensation, but they are also extra sensitive to the development of depression. The question is how brain development is related to this.

Adolescence

Adolescence is the transition from childhood to adulthood. Adolescence starts around the start of puberty and ends when the individual is relatively independent of the parents. This means that the individual can live alone.

Looking at the past, adolescence is seen as a turbulent period. In this period there is a much larger amount of preventable deaths and there is a relatively large amount of psychopathology.

Understanding adolescence

There are two main approaches to understanding adolescents' behaviour and brain development:

  • Translation. Behaviour that belongs to adolescence, such as increased time spent with friends, is investigated here. The age-related behaviour entails risks, but also provides important adjustment functions. The changed behavioural pattern associated with adolescence is not specific to adolescents but occurs in many mammals. Because adolescence is an important phase from an evolutionary point of view, animal studies can also contribute to our understanding.

  • Transition. As mentioned, adolescence is a transition period between childhood and adulthood. Many different stages of development have been investigated by themselves, with professionals focusing on the young child, for example. However, it is difficult to understand whether a certain process belongs to adolescence when other developmental stages are not considered. The interaction between the various development stages is interesting here.

Various developmental steps are indicated in the article:

  • Development non-specific for adolescence

  • Development occurring in adolescence

  • Development typical of adolescence

Note that the corresponding graphs can also be displayed the other way around: for example, development typically shows a peak during adolescence, but there may also be a trough. Non-specific development can increase linearly, as shown in the figure, but can also decrease linearly.

Neurobiological models

Some neurobiological models are shown in the article:

  • Dual system model. The two systems model of willpower forms the basis for this model. According to this model, self-monitoring is the result of the balance between a cold and a warm system. The cool system is emotion-neutral, strategic and flexible, while the warm system is driven by fear, wishes and reflexes. Individual development and stress affect the balance between the two systems.

This model can contribute to our understanding of direct and deferred remuneration. In addition, the dual system model can be used to explain the difference between adolescents and adults in looking for sensation and making risky decisions.

  • Triadic model of motivated behaviour. This model explains the differences between adolescents and adults. The limbic (emotional) system is subdivided into two parts:

    • reward (ventral striatum)

    • avoidance (amygdala)

According to this model, motivated behaviour arises from a balance of reward-driven and damage-avoiding behaviour. In adolescence, there is a stronger tendency towards reward-driven behaviour and there is, therefore, an imbalance.

  • Imbalance model. Regional neurochemical, structural and functional changes in the brain cause an imbalance in brain circuits. Different brain regions mature earlier and faster than other brain regions. For example, the sensorimotor and subcortical areas are matured faster than the prefrontal cortex. In addition, the sensorimotor cortex reaches its peak in cortical volume in late childhood, while the association cortexes only reach their peak volume in adolescence. The dopamine system, involved in reward, also has a density peak of receptors in early adolescence in the striatum, while this peak in the prefrontal cortex is only reached in adulthood.

The above striking differences are not found in childhood since relatively many brain areas have not yet matured. In adulthood, however, a relatively large number of brain regions have matured, through which even such differences are no longer present.

Self-control

Definition

Self-control is being able to suppress inappropriate emotions, wishes and actions and to display appropriate behaviour instead. A classic example of this is resisting a direct reward to receive a larger reward later (delay of gratification or the Marshmallow task).

The underlying mechanism

The article identifies three areas of the brain that are important for the cognitive and motivational circuit that supports self-control:

  • Amygdala. Important for associative learning and determining the value of emotional cues from the environment. This structure can additionally activate the striatum or inhibit it.

  • Prefrontal cortex. Here lies the capacity for reasoning and behavioural regulation. This structure modulates the other two structures to suppress actions guided by emotion.

  • Ventral striatum. This structure has to do with learning and predicting rewards.

These three structures are modulated by dopamine and the hippocampus.

The brain of the adolescent differs in many ways from that of children and adults. In adolescents, for example, there is reduced top-down regulation of increased emotional responses. In addition, motivated actions are supported by the reduced top-down modulation of the prefrontal cortex.

Incentive for self-monitoring

Self-control seems to increase linearly from childhood to adulthood. Nevertheless, something can be said about this: in the case of incitement, self-control changes. For example, a reward for performance may result in us doing better next time. There are person-specific differences in this area. Individuals can to a greater or lesser extent shift their focus from direct information to a focus on later pay.

Attractive cues

One of the first investigations into reward processing looked at differences in the level of financial remuneration. Both the ventral striatum and the orbitofrontal cortex were found to be sensitive to indications that predicted the greatest reward. This sensitivity was much higher in adolescents than in children and adults.

Adolescents appear to take more risk when they receive direct feedback than adults. Adolescents have a peak in reward sensitivity around the age of fifteen.

Adolescents are less good at suppressing a response to an unexpected positive cue compared to children and adults. This can be seen neurologically in increased activity in the ventral striatum in adolescents.

Incentives based on performance

In research, participants were told that for some achievements, if they were performed properly, they received a financial reward. The expectation of a reward led to a greater improvement in performance in adolescents than in adults. This is represented in the brain as increased activation in the ventral striatum in adolescents. Unfortunately, baseline measures were not taken into account in these studies. Perhaps adults were already performing so well that it was virtually impossible for them to improve their performance in anticipation of a reward. In addition, the reward for adolescents may be subjectively higher for adolescents than for adults.

Research with a point system instead of a financial reward had to circumvent this last problem. This research showed that adolescents with relatively high rewards are good at not responding impulsively.

Social environment

The social environment influences behaviour throughout life. It seems that the influence of the social environment during adolescence is by far the greatest. For example, a large increase in risky behaviour can be observed in adolescents, and not in adults, when peers are present. This can be indirectly associated with the dopamine-rich part of the ventral striatum. It seems that the presence of peers has a behavioural effect. A study into the extent to which peers ensure behavioural reinforcement has been investigated. Every form of positive reinforcement led to faster response time. In adolescents, there was an increased activation pattern in the premotor circuit with positive social feedback, regardless of the outcome. This was not observed in adults and children.

Self-Control in the event of a threat

Adolescents are not fearless: they overestimate the chance of a negative outcome after risky behaviour. Yet they cannot take this into account sufficiently at the moment, probably due to a combination of peers, the environment and their own emotions.

Indications of danger

The best indication of danger comes from the frightened facial expression of another person. Research shows that we learn fears at an early age. How we can express and suppress this depends on development. The first neuroimaging studies on anxiety focused on the amygdala, the structure that external cues must interpret on emotional value. When a fearful cue is found, the individual is prepared to fight or flee (or even freeze). The lateral nucleus projects onto the central nucleus. This then projects onto the brainstem, the hypothalamus, and the autonomic nervous system. This causes the expression of fear. When the danger cue is no longer present, the fear response is suppressed.

Research into the amygdala among adolescents and adults indicates that adolescents have a greater activation pattern with possible danger expressions. Follow-up research indicates that adolescents also have increased activation compared to children. Adolescents also respond more slowly to anxious cues than to neutral or cheerful cues.

Recent studies have shown that adolescents find it difficult to suppress responses to emotional stimuli. Adolescents, especially men, respond more impulsively to threatening cues than neutral cues compared to children and adults. This can also be seen from increased activity in the limbic cortical areas and the ventral striatum.

The increased level of impulsivity in adolescents could be the result of a not yet sufficiently mature prefrontal top-down regulation of the amygdala.

Conditioned fear

The traditional conditioning process involves the combination of a neutral cue with a negative stimulus. After repeated combining, the neutral cue receives a negative charge. Research into conditioned anxiety among children, adolescents and adults showed that adolescents appeared to be less able to extinguish conditioned fear than children and adults. Similar results have been found in research with mice.

Adolescence can be seen as a period in which anxiety associations are less well regulated, which can lead to reduced self-control when the anxiety is present.

Contextual fear

Contextual anxiety depends on being able to learn from threats in the environment. Research into this has been done among mice. Mice in the adolescence period seemed to suffer little from context-dependent anxiety, compared to younger and adult mice. What is special is that when the adolescent mice grow up, they do show the context-dependent fear (which they had already been taught in adolescence) as adult mice. An explanation may be that the amygdala activity has been dulled by changes in the hippocampus during context-dependent conditioning.

Conclusion

The ability to suppress inappropriate emotions, wishes and actions and replace them with appropriate behaviour diminished when striking environmental cues are present. Both in behaviour and in the brain this leads to an increased reactivity in adolescents compared to children and adults.

Why are the brains programmed that way?

It seems logical that brain mechanisms have evolved around socially relevant cues since social status in evolution is very important for survival.

The adolescence period is by no means unique to humans but is observed in many different mammals. During adolescence, it is important to acquire skills to survive independently of others in adulthood. The search for new things means that new sources can be explored and new (types of) relationships can be entered into. Everything leads to the ultimate goal of adolescence: becoming an adult.

The changes in adolescents' development are helped because adolescents are extra sensitive to socially important cues and are less troubled by potential dangers. Together this causes exploration in the world, beyond the safe environment of a home.

Mental health

One in five adolescents has a mental illness, for example, substance abuse. Early substance abuse is a good predictor of later addiction. In addition, narcotics affect the dopamine system. This reinforces the rewarding effect of the dopamine system, which is very active in adolescence.

In addition to substance abuse, there are also many adolescents (around 10 per cent) with an anxiety disorder. This is because being unable to suppress an emotional response when no danger threatens becomes pathological. Adolescents with an anxiety disorder have increased activity in the amygdala and there is a decreased connectivity in the front-amygdala circuit. Desensitization is the most common treatment for an anxiety disorder. This technique works with fear cancellation. Unfortunately, this form of treatment only works in half of the patients. The studies discussed earlier in this article seem to indicate that anxiety killing for adolescents is not the right way to reduce anxiety disorder.

Laws and policies

In recent years, an enormous number of laws and policy documents have been amended in juvenile criminal law. The studies aimed at the development of adolescents and the increased tendency towards risky behaviour and impulsiveness were used. This research indicates that young people should be held responsible for their behaviour, but that there should be a shared responsibility.

Further neuroimaging research into the brain-behavioural relationship in adolescents can make a major contribution to the legal system:

  • Immature brain structures during adolescence affect judgment, decision-making, risk-taking, and criminal behaviour.

  • Research can contribute to the treatment and rehabilitation of criminal youth.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Navigating the social environment in adolescence the role of social brain development by Andrews et al. - 2020 - Chapter

Article summary of Navigating the social environment in adolescence the role of social brain development by Andrews et al. - 2020 - Chapter

What is this article about?

In this article, research on the development of social cognitive processes and structural and functional changes in the social brain during adolescence is reviewed.

As individuals age, the brain changes in terms of gray and white matter volume, surface area, and cortical thickness. During childhood, cortical gray matter increases and in late childhood this reaches a peak. Later it declines, and it stabilizes as individuals reach their mid-20s. There is also a linear increase in white matter volume across childhood and adolescence.

Brain development also seems to be influenced by sex hormones, which control the onset of and progression through puberty. For instance, pubertal maturation is related to developmental changes in subcortical brain volume. The neural and psychosocial changes that are related to puberty can also lead to mental health vulnerabilities. The latest maturing brain regions are within the ‘social brain’, which is related to recognizing, understanding, and interpreting social cues from others. These include the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, posterior superior temporal sulcus, anterior temporal cortex, amygdala, and anterior insula. Some of these regions are also involved in ‘mentalizing’, which is the ability to interpret the mental states of others. Studies have shown that regions within this social brain show a protracted structural development. For instance, the amygdala increases with 7% in volume between late childhood and mid-adolescence. After 14 years of age, there are no significant changes.

During adolescence, there are also significant functional maturations of regions that are involved in social cognitive processes. This means that during adolescence, mentalizing, social cognition, executive functioning, and emotion regulation skills all develop until adulthood is reached.

What do we know about mentalizing?

Mentalizing is the ability to understand and predict other people’s behavior. It has been suggested that adolescents, relative to adults, use different cognitive strategies when they think about other’s intentions.

Taking other people’s perspectives is important, especially when integrating into new social contexts (which happens during puberty), and when choosing peers to be friends with. One study has shown that both adolescents and adults recruit the dmPFC when taking someone else’s perspective into account, but when there were no social cues present, only adolescents also recruited the dmPFC. This means that adolescents also recruit mentalizing brain regions, when this not required.

The ability to mentalize is still maturing during adolescence, with children and adolescents making more errors than adults on perspective-taking tasks. Inhibitory control has also been pointed to as a contributor to these differences, which is also still maturing during adolescence.

It has also been shown that adolescents who perform poor on perspective taking tasks are more likely to report loneliness and peer rejection. In one study, reduced mentalizing was also related with the severity of depression. According to the stress-reward-mentalizing model, child and adolescent depression emerges from the interaction among impairments in stress-regulatory, reward, and mentalizing systems.

What about emotion regulation?

Adolescents who are accepted by their peers show more adaptive emotion regulation and are at lower risk for internalizing symptoms. The protracted development of the PFC (important for emotion regulation) may lead to that adolescents are less able to regulate their emotions, which increases their risk for anxiety- and stress-related disorders. Emotion regulation thus develops with across adolescence. The ability to mentalize is an important factor in the development of adequate emotion regulation abilities. Individual variations in emotion regulation may contribute to risky decision making in the presence of peers. Poor emotion regulation is also associated with more participation in risky behaviors.

What about the social risk of rejection?

Adolescents are more likely to take risks than adults when peers are present, and they are also hypersensitive to social rejection, and they are more likely to take health and legal risks in the presence of peers, to avoid this social rejection. Social rejection has been related to disruptions in emotion regulations, and is a risk factor for adolescent-onset mood disorders such as depression and anxiety. For example, adolescents can engage in smoking when this is the peer group norm, to avoid the risk of rejection, even when there are health and legal consequences. This is not always irrational; sometimes it does really help to engage in these risky behaviors to be accepted by the peer group.

What about peer influence on prosocial behavior?

Peers can also have a positive influence on prosocial decisions. For example, when adolescents observe that their peers volunteer, they are more likely to also volunteer. Adolescents are also more likely to be influenced by others toward prosocial behaviors compared to adults. High-status peers and close friends are more influential. Prosocial risk taking, for example standing up for someone who is being bullied, is not related to harmful risk-taking such as reckless driving and drug taking. Instead, it is associated with lower reward sensitivity, higher punishment sensitivity, and greater school engagement. Thus, positive risk taking seems to be beneficial for adolescents.

What can be concluded?

Adolescents are thus vulnerable to mental health problems, and risk factors in their social environment such as peer rejection contributes to this vulnerability. Interventions that aim to improve the vulnerability toward poor mental health may focus on improving prosocial behavior and emotion regulation abilities.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of A neurocognitive perspective on the development of social decision-making by Will & Güroğlu - 2016 - Chapter

Article summary of A neurocognitive perspective on the development of social decision-making by Will & Güroğlu - 2016 - Chapter

What is this article about?

Humans are highly social compared to other species. Even 1-year-old toddlers help others. This social behaviour keeps developing and becomes increasingly complex with age. For example, preschoolers mainly use lies for their own benefits, while school-aged children start to lie to protect other’s feelings (white lies).

Developmental changes in social behaviour are related to developmental changes in cognitive functions, such as perspective taking and impulse control. In the current chapter, evidence for the hypothesis that the gradual development of impulse control and perspective-taking skills are related with more strategic thinking and increased incorporation of other’s intentions in social decision-making.

Why use economic games?

Economic games were introduced by behavioural economics. These are used to study psychological and neural mechanisms that underlie social decision-making. Examples are the Ultimatum Game and the Dictator Game. In these games, there are two players. In the Ultimatum Game, one player is given a reward (money, food or anything else). This person is the ‘proposer’, and proposes to split his reward with another player, the ‘responder’. If the responder accepts the proposal, both players receive what was proposed. If the responder rejects, then none of the players receives any reward. In the Dictator Game, the responder is not able to reject the proposal, and thus only receives the amount that the proposer transfers. An assumption of Game theoretical models is that all players should accept all proposals that are above 0, because any free reward would be beneficial. However, this is not true. These games have shown that people do not only care about maximizing their outcomes, they also seem to care about others. For example, in the Dictator Game, the proposer often transfers on average 20-30% of their stakes to the responder, even though they do not have to. In the Ultimatum Game, proposers often choose for an equal split of their stake and responders usually reject offers that are smaller than 20% of the stake. It has also been shown that people do not like to receive less than the proposer, which is called disadvantageous inequity aversion. When people then reject the offer, this might serve as a way to ‘correct’ the inequity.

These paradigms are useful, because they can be used to determine differences between age groups. They also make it easier to quantify complex social behavior which is useful for neuroimaging research. Furthermore, these games can be manipulated so that researchers can gain more understanding of the subcomponents of social decision-making.

How does a preference for fairness develop?

Toddlers are reluctant to engage in prosocial behavior when it is costly to themselves. Studies employing the Dictator Game have shown that although children tend to keep most of the resources to themselves, the size of their donations increase between the ages of 3 and 8. By the age of 9, their donations do not differ from donations made by adults. Thus, there is an increase in costly sharing. This is not due to differences in knowledge about what is fair; infants as young as 15 months already expect resources to be distributed equally. Even though 3-year-olds do not differ from 8-year-olds in beliefs about equal division of rewards, they still keep more than 50% of the rewards to themselves in a Dictator Game. During this age, the willingness to send rewards increases. This means that a developing sense of fairness leads to that children engage in more equal splits.

In addition to fairness considerations, there are also strategic concerns. For instance, adults offer higher shares of the stake when they know that the second player can punish (reject) unfair offers. One can thus look at differences in the Ultimatum Game and Dictator Game as a measure of strategic social behavior. During late childhood (7-10 years), children make higher Ultimatum Game proposals compared to Dictator Game proposals, but their proposals are still smaller than those proposed by adults. During adolescence, the difference between the two games becomes greater, which means that there is a developmental increase in strategic behavior across adolescence. In sum, the prosocial tendency to share with others emerges early, but social behavior becomes increasingly strategic across childhood and adolescence.

What are the cognitive mechanisms underlying developmental change in strategic social behavior?

It has been shown that strategic social behavior is dependent on the capacity to implement behavioral control; a selfish impulse (keeping all resources to oneself) has to be controlled. Proposers also need to be able to take the responder’s perspective into account, to predict what proposals they might accept or reject. This perspective-taking is often called ‘theory of mind’, which young children often are not capable of. In sum, behavioral studies that have employed these game paradigms show that cognitive development which are both related to impulse control and perspective taking play a crucial role in understanding age-related changes in social behavior.

What neural networks are involved in social decision-making?

There are three brain networks which are important in social decision-making: a basic affective network, a cognitive regulatory network, and a ‘mentalizing’ network. The basic affective network consists of the anterior insula, ventral striatum, and the amygdala. These determine whether social stimuli should be approached (positive affect) or avoided (negative affect). Increasing activity in the insula has been related to unreciprocated trust and receiving unfair offers. These brain regions interact with a cognitive regulatory network which consists of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and regions in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), such as the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vIPFC) and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). These areas are related to cognitive control over impulses and can help individuals to act in a goal-directed manner when they experience a conflict between self-interest and social norms. These control-related areas are thus crucial for the regulation of strategic social behavior. Both the affective and control regions interact with a third system, the ‘mentalizing’ network. This includes the left and right temporoparietal junction (TPJ), superior temporal sulci, ventral, and dorsal regions of the medial PFC and the temporal poles. These regions are often active in tasks that involve thinking about other people’s mental states, when people have to infer other people’s thoughts, beliefs or desires. Lastly, taking other people’s perspectives in economic games has been associated with activation in regions of the mentalizing network, such as the TPJ and the dorsomedial PFC.

How can we understand social behavior from a developmental neuroscience perspective?

It has been shown that different brain regions mature at different ages. Sensorimotor regions in the occipital and parietal lobes mature first. The dlPFC and the TPJ are the latest brain regions to fully mature, which might explain the protracted developmental pattern in associated functions such as cognitive control and perspective taking. Affective networks mature early, while regions of the cognitive regulatory network mature late. This can explain why affective reactions to unfairness are visible early in development and why strategic considerations during social decision-making develops later. This is in line with studies that have shown that detection of violations of fairness and neural responses in the insula and the dACC mature before adolescence, and that intentionality understanding in fairness decisions develops across adolescence and is related with neural activity that are important for perspective taking (TPJ) and impulse control (dlPFC). It has been suggested that early adolescents tend to make more self-oriented choices, while late adolescents tend to make more other-oriented choices. This is due to differences in the development of the dmPFC and the TPJ.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Time spent with friends in adolescence relates to less neural sensitivity to later peer rejection by Masten et al. - Chapter

Article summary of Time spent with friends in adolescence relates to less neural sensitivity to later peer rejection by Masten et al. - Chapter

Introduction

During the adolescence period, peers become increasingly important, adolescents also spend more and more of their time with their peers. This changed use of time entails a major disadvantage: the chance of rejection is increased. On the other hand, peers also offer benefits such as social support. This can then reinforce adolescents against rejection by others. The more social support an adolescent receives, the less adverse effects he or she experiences after being rejected by peers. The underlying mechanisms for this effect are still unknown.

The current study looks at the neurobiological background of friends during adolescence and rejection by peers at a later age. A recent idea is that people who receive more social support experience social stressors as less threatening.

The dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula are associated with the affective response to negative social experiences, such as social exclusion. With negative social interactions, people with many friends should, therefore, show less activity in these two structures. A recent study showed that young adults who have daily contact with close friends show less activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in social exclusion. The current study extends this finding to the question of whether social contacts during adolescence have the same effect when social exclusion only occurs later (in young adulthood).

Friendship during adolescence was measured in the current study as the amount of time the adolescent spends with friends (outside of school) using a diary. Two years later, the participants were examined with an fMRI scan while they were excluded by two peers. The prediction was that when adolescents spent many years with peers, they would be less sensitive to social exclusion in young adulthood.

Methods

Attendees

Research was conducted among 21 participants, 13 of whom were women. They were in the last year of their high school (12th grade). About half of the participants were white and the other half were Hispano.

Procedure

The participants had to fill in their diaries every evening for two weeks. In it, they stated the amount of time they had spent outside school with their friends. The fMRI scan was performed about two years later.

fMRI

Cyberball was used to create the illusion of social exclusion. The participant thinks that he is replacing a ball with two others that he had met before the fMRI scan (a man and a woman). The participant was able to decide for himself to whom he threw the ball, the decisions of the other two people were made by the computer. After the participant was involved in the first ten throws, he was excluded from possession for the rest of the game.

All participants also performed a control task prior to the Cyberball game. Hereby they looked at an asterisk (=star) that in a triangular form closely resembled the Cyberball game.

Analysis schedule

The Cyberball game and the control task were executed in a block design. Group contrasts were used to measure neural activity. This involved looking at neural activity during the period of exclusion during the Cyberball game and during the control task.

Regression analysis (Region of Interest, ROI) was used to measure the relationship between time spent with friends in adolescence and brain activity (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula) in social exclusion. In addition, a regression analysis was performed on the activity in the entire brain during the social exclusion and the time spent with friends in adolescence.

Results

Behavioural analysis

Adolescents spent 0 to 4.47 hours a day with their friends. There was no gender difference, but there was a difference in origin: white participants spent more time with friends than Hispanics. All statistical analyzes, therefore, checked for origin.

The main effect on the entire brain

During social exclusion, there was greater brain activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the left and right anterior insula compared to the control condition. In addition, there was also increased activity in different brain areas involved in emotion regulation.

ROI

There appeared to be a negative relationship between the amount of time an adolescent spent with friends and the activity in the anterior insula during social exclusion (the more time spent with friends, the less activity in the anterior insula). No association was found with activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex.

Whole brain

The relationship between friends in adolescence and reduced brain activity in young adulthood in social exclusion related only to two areas in the left anterior insula and an area just behind the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, still associated with this structure, but not included in the ROI analysis. There appeared to be no effect on other brain regions.

Discussion

A larger amount of time spent with friends during adolescence appears to be related to a reduced sensitivity to social exclusion during young adulthood. This contributes to our knowledge about friendships during adolescence and their long-term effects. This finding also confirms results from previous research. For example, it has been shown that reduced pain-related neuronal reactions occur with social exclusion (adults) when there is a lot of contact with supportive others.

In addition to the above, the findings in the current study appear to be evidence of the protective effect of friends during adolescence in the long term. However, more research is needed here. The finding that friendship mediates the degree of social stress is also supported by current research.

However, the results of the current study are not entirely clear: friendship during adolescence, for example, could result in people becoming less sensitive to negative social events, rather than an improved ability to regulate affective responses to social stress.

Rejection by peers is less threatening for adolescents with many friends:

  • They know they have reliable friends, so rejection by a peer is not a bad thing.

  • They know they are accepted by many peers, so rejection is not threatening to this acceptance.

  • Rejection increases the need to be in contact with someone. An adolescent with many friends has an increased chance of this.

  • Friends who witness the rejection often help the adolescent.

Limitations and future research

This study looked at the amount of time an adolescent spent with peers. The same research could be performed with other variables, such as the number of friends or the quality of the friendship. In addition, stability could also be considered: can the adolescent maintain a friendship for a longer period of time and what effects does this have on social exclusion?

In the current study, it has been decided to measure the time spent with peers and social exclusion both at one time. The change in this over time could, therefore, be an interesting research topic.

In addition, the study did not look at current friendships either. It cannot, therefore, be stated with certainty that friendships in adolescence have achieved this effect; it could be that an adolescent who spends a lot of time with friends still does this in adulthood. Research that takes this into account is therefore desirable.

The two areas of the brain that were specifically investigated in the current study were selected on the basis of earlier research. However, this does not mean that only these brain regions are involved. The underlying mechanisms are also not disclosed.

Other biological and neurochemical mechanisms should also be further investigated.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game donations during adolescence by Van Hoorn et al. - 2016 - Chapter

Article summary of Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game donations during adolescence by Van Hoorn et al. - 2016 - Chapter

Introduction

Adolescence is seen as the transition period between childhood and adulthood. During this period there are major changes in cognitive and social-affective reasoning. Adolescents are very sensitive to social events and the influence of peers. The cognitive changes in adolescence seem to originate from the changes in the social cognitive network in the brain during the same period. The medial prefrontal cortex, the temporoparietal junction and the superior temporal sulcus are examples of brain regions that undergo a major change in both functioning and structure during adolescence.

Several studies have shown that adolescents show more risky behaviour when they are in the presence of peers. This seems to be related to brain areas involved in the affective processing of risks and rewards. These areas are more active when peers are present, especially among adolescents.

Few studies have been conducted into the influence of peers in a more positive way. Recent studies seem to indicate that adolescents are becoming more pro-social when peers are around. Current research also builds on this notion and also wants to look at the areas of the brain involved in prosocial behaviour in adolescence. Adult research indicates that the medial prefrontal cortex and the ventral striatum play a role in peer evaluation. The medial prefrontal cortex also seems more active when a social situation is only imagined.

Current study

Research was done in two age groups (12-13 and 15-16 years). They were compared with each other regarding the effects of peer influence. Previous research suggests that young adolescents are more sensitive to this.

The Public Goods Game was used during this investigation. In this social dilemma game, the participant must distribute tokens among themselves and the group. Three conditions were used to assess peers:

  • Spectators (actors of similar age) who promoted prosocial behaviour (many tokens for the group: thumbs up).

  • Spectators (actors of similar age) who did nothing.

  • No spectators.

The spectators and the participants met before the game started. The researchers' prediction was that there should be more activity in the medial prefrontal cortex when spectators were present in young adolescents than in middle adolescents.

Methods

Attendees

The 12-13-year-olds age group included 31 participants, 15 of whom were men. The age group 15-16-year-olds contained 30 participants, including 14 men. More than 90% of the participants were born in the Netherlands. All participants spoke and wrote Dutch fluently. An IQ measurement was done with the WISC-III and the WAIS-III. All IQ scores were within the normal range and there were no significant differences in the age groups.

Experimental research

The participants were told that they were playing the public goods game online with four anonymous other players of the same age. These were also participants in the study. The anonymity would remain guaranteed. Each round the participants received five tokens, with values ​​of 1, 1.50 or 2 euros per token. All tokens given to the group were doubled and then distributed among the group members. This means that the participant has the most money left if he keeps all tokens to himself, while the group benefits the most when each participant donates all tokens to the group. Donating to the group can be seen as prosocial behaviour. The participants could not see what the others were doing and did not get to see the results per round.

72 trials were used, divided into two rounds and three conditions:

 

Spectators with feedback

Spectators without feedback

No spectators

round 1

12 trials

12 trials

12 trials

round 2

12 trials

12 trials

12 trials

For each block of 12 trials, the participant was shown for two seconds in which condition they were. When participants received feedback at the end of the trial, they saw 0 thumbs up at 0 and 1 donated tokens, 2 thumbs up at 2 donated tokens, 4 thumbs up at 3 donated tokens and 5 thumbs up at 4 and 5 tokens. When participants had spectators but received no feedback, the participants were told that the spectators did evaluate, but their feedback would not be visible.

The spectators consisted of 44 actors, who changed per trial session. The goal was to introduce all participants to six out of ten actors, which succeeded in 75% of the cases. During the game itself, the participants saw a photo of the actors with neutral facial expression.

Procedure

This research was part of a larger research into relationships with peers. The participants arrived at this study with their best friend, who participated in another part of the study. The participant heard that it was necessary to wait for more participants, after which the (usually 6) actors came in a little later. The participant was introduced to them. The participant would be examined in the game in an MRI scanner. He or she then heard that not all other 'participants' were there yet, but that the MRI procedures would already be explained. The participant already played five practice sessions there. It was also explicitly stated that the participant would not play against the best friend. The scanning session lasted approximately one hour. Afterwards, the participants completed questionnaires and did two sub-tests of the WISC or the WAIS. This was followed by a debriefing and a compensation of 32 euros per participant.

Analytical methods

Two ANOVAs were performed to investigate responses to the stimulus and feedback onset. A 3x3 ANOVA (3 conditions and 3 token values) was used for the stimulus onset and a 3x3 ANOVA for the feedback onset. In addition, a regression analysis was performed on data across the entire brain.

Regions of Interest (ROI) analyses were also carried out to see the differences between the two age groups.

Results

Behavioural analysis

Participants donated more tokens to the group when there were spectators. Most tokens were donated to conditional viewers with feedback. The tokens that were most frequently donated were the tokens with a value of one euro. The tokens with a value of two euros were the least donated. In every condition, more was donated to the group by young adolescents than by middle adolescents.

fMRI analyses at the stimulus onset

The stimulus onset is the moment that the participant decides how he or she will distribute the tokens. The condition had a major effect on various areas of the brain, including the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, the precuneus superior temporal sulcus and the temporoparietal junction. The value of the token had no effect on the brain and there was also no interaction effect.

When spectators were present, there was more activation in the precuneus, the bilateral temporoparietal junction and the bilateral superior temporal sulcus, compared to the condition without spectators.

When spectators who gave feedback were present, there was also increased activation in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, compared to the condition without spectators.

There was no significant interaction effect when looking at the age group. To get a better picture of this, we looked at the ROI analyses. This showed that the young adolescents showed a greater activation difference in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the left superior temporal sulcus in the condition viewer with feedback and no viewer than the middle adolescents.

Regression looked at activation in the brain and height of the donated tokens. In the conditions with feedback-giving spectators versus no spectators, more activation appeared to be in the left temporoparietal junction with a higher donation.

FMRI analyses with the feedback onset

This concerns the analyses of brain activation when the feedback screen is displayed. The repeated ANOVA measurements on the condition and token value show the main effect of condition in many different brain regions, such as the bilateral insula and the right amygdala. There was no main effect for token value and no interaction effect. When the spectator conditions were compared with the no spectator condition, there was additional brain activation in more areas in the important areas that have to do with face recognition at the spectator conditions compared to the no spectator condition. In the condition viewer with feedback, compared to the condition viewer without feedback, increased activity was visible in different brain areas,such as the bilateral insula and the right superior parietal cortex.

There appeared to be no interaction effect between age group and condition. When ROI analyses looked at this extra closely, there appeared to be an interaction effect: the young adolescents had larger activation differences between the viewer with feedback and the viewer without feedback condition than the middle adolescents.

Discussion

The social behaviour of adolescents is characterized by a high sensitivity to the opinion of peers. Current research has shown that peer feedback has a positive effect on the degree of prosocial behaviour during early and middle adolescence. If the feedback from peers is also prosocial in nature (thumbs up), the effects are greatest. Similar results have been found in adults in other studies.

To make a choice to donate in the presence of peers, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (involved in social influence), the temporoparietal junction and the superior temporal sulcus become active. The current research showed that prosocial feedback had little influence on the level of activity in these brain regions, only the idea of ​​peer evaluation is sufficient for the increased activity.

Other research into the influence of peers also shows extra activation in the ventral striatum. An explanation for this is that the presence of peers makes behaviour more rewarding. This activation has not been demonstrated in the current study.

Current research has found that prosocial behaviour (donating a higher amount) entails greater brain activity in the temporoparietal junction when the participant was assessed with feedback. This brain structure appears to be involved in altruism.

From a behavioural perspective, younger participants gave a higher amount to the group than older participants. This applied to all conditions, including the condition without peers. This is in line with other studies that indicate that younger adolescents are more prosocial towards strangers. Young adolescents also seem to be the most sensitive to the opinions of peers. This was also found in the brain in the current study: there was greater brain activation among young adolescents when spectators were present.

Learning and cognitive control through the presence of peers with feedback could play a role in regulating behaviour and adaptation to others. More research is needed here.

In the current study, no distinction was made between the amount of the donation and the feedback that was given (the feedback was purely geared to the number of tokens, not to the value thereof). This has consequences for the interpretation of the results found.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry by Chein et al. - 2011 - Chapter

Article summary of Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry by Chein et al. - 2011 - Chapter

Introduction

Adolescents behave more at risk than children or adults. This risky behaviour can be characterized as behaviour that can be prevented (for example, binge drinking or traffic accidents due to wild driving). Many professionals state that this behaviour poses the greatest threat to the well-being of adolescents. The risky behaviour that adolescents display differs from the risky behaviour of adults in terms of social background (adolescents often behave more at risk when peers are present) and the quantity. Recent research shows that the risky behaviour of adolescents is directly influenced by the presence of peers: as soon as peers are present, adolescents are more at risk. This has not been found in adults. It appears that two neuronal systems are involved in the tendency toward risky behaviour in adolescents:

  • The spur processing system (ventral striatum including the nucleus accumbens and the orbitofrontal cortex).

  • The cognitive control system (lateral prefrontal cortex).

Previous research into impulsive and risky behaviour has often observed increased activity in the brain areas involved in incentive processing. In addition, studies found reduced activity in brain structures involved in cognitive control. Both brain systems are subject to many changes during adolescence. The first system undergoes many changes, particularly in the early adolescence period, due to the distribution and density of dopamine receptors. This also explains the greater sensitivity to reward in this period.

The cognitive control system matures more slowly and also takes longer to do so. This is partly due to the reduction of grey matter and the increase of myelin layers. This process starts before the onset of adolescence and lasts until at least the 25th year of life. Due to maturation, adolescents are becoming better at positions related to executive functioning and flexibility.

It seems that the risky behaviour of adolescents is caused by an imbalance between the maturation of the aforementioned systems. In the current study, adolescents, young adults and adults measured brain activity when making decisions in a simulated traffic light task. The ultimate goal of this game is to achieve the highest possible financial reward in the shortest possible time. Every participant played the game with and without peers looking at them.

Methods

Attendees

40 people participated in this study: 14 adolescents between 14-18 years (including 6 women), 14 young adults between 19-22 years (including 7 women) and 12 adults between 24-29 years (including 6 women).

Procedure

The traffic light task is a driving task where the participant must keep control of the vehicle from the driver's position. Each participant did the game four times (twice with, and twice without spectators). Each round had twenty intersections (so there were twenty trials per round) and lasted less than six minutes. By pressing a button, the participant could indicate whether or not to stop at the intersection, while the traffic light went from green to orange to red. By varying the timing of the traffic lights and the chance of a collision when driving through red, the game remained unpredictable. Risky behaviour was rewarded financially (by a higher amount if the participant was faster). Waiting for green light resulted in less time due to the time delay. However, when the risky behaviour caused a collision, the loss of time was greater (and the financial reward smaller) than if the participant waited for the green light.

Each participant had to bring two friends of the same gender and in the same age group. In the spectator condition, the participant was told that his or her friends were watching in an adjacent room. This entailed a surprise effect since the participants were not informed of this in advance. During the break for the spectator condition and between the two spectator rounds, the participant and the spectators were able to speak to each other via the intercom. The peers were instructed about what they were allowed to say and what they were not allowed to say to prevent behavioural influences.

After the traffic light task (where the participant was in an MRI scanner), the participants were asked to complete questionnaires. These questionnaires investigated the degree of impulsiveness, sensation seeking and sensitivity to the influence of peers.

Results

Behavioural

Adolescents and (young) adults showed similar behaviour in the condition without spectators. However, this was not the case in the condition with spectators. The participants in the adolescent group were the only ones who took more risk when their friends watched. They drove more often through red and also had an accident more often.

Regression analysis showed that behaviour was significantly related to the degree of sensation seeking. There was no significant relationship between behaviour and impulsivity. Task performance is therefore mainly influenced by the extent to which someone seeks to reward and sensation.

fMRI

In the current study, particular attention was paid to those areas of the brain, which left the main effect of age or an interaction effect of age and condition.

In areas in the left prefrontal cortex, there was more brain activity in adults than in adolescents.

The influence of peers on adolescents was evident in increased activation in the ventral striatum and the orbitofrontal cortex. This indicates that the presence of peers makes the incentive processing more sensitive. These results were not found in (young) adults.

In addition, different regions of interests (ROIs) were looked at to make the decision-making process dependent on the riskiness of the given situation. In adolescents, there was more activation in the ventral striatum and the orbitofrontal cortex, related to risky decisions. This was not found with the (young) adults.

The self-report of sensitivity to peers turned out to be significantly related to the activation patterns in the ventral striatum. This applied to all age groups. 

Discussion

The results from the current study replicate earlier research findings. The unique way of measuring the influence of peers in the current study contributes to a better understanding of the effect of peers' influence on the decision-making process during adolescence. The riskier behaviour when peers looked at it cannot be explained by the encouragement of these peers, as there was no contact during the task and the contact between the tasks was subject to strict rules. An explanation that may be possible is a neural sensitivity due to the incomplete maturation of the brain.

The difference in risky behaviour between adolescents and (young) adults in the current study can only be attributed to the presence of peers since all three groups showed similar behaviour in the condition where no spectators were present. In adults, the presence of peers does not even seem to be rewarding at all. In addition, they appear to be better able to use the left prefrontal cortex to inhibit the reward system to make strategic decisions. Regardless of the presence of peers, there was more activation in the aforementioned brain area in adults than in adolescents.

The social influence on decisions made by adolescents can be seen as an interaction between social information processing and the processing of reward.

Adolescents have increased activation in the brain areas involved in incentive processing in response to social stimuli such as face recognition, compared to children and adults. Because of the rewarding effects of the presence of peers, this can lead to a reinforcing stimulus towards the incentive processing system, whereby risky behaviour is experienced as extra rewarding.

It cannot be claimed with certainty that the increased activation in the ventral striatum in adolescents in the current study is purely due to the presence of peers. An alternative explanation can be given, for example, by talking about social uncertainty or greater sensitivity to distraction. These explanations are less likely since other brain regions should also show different activation patterns during the current study.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Adolescent-specific patterns of behavior and neural activity during social reinforcement learning by Jones et al. - 2014 - Chapter

Article summary of Adolescent-specific patterns of behavior and neural activity during social reinforcement learning by Jones et al. - 2014 - Chapter

What is this article about?

Humans need to be able to decipher and learn from social signals. Especially during adolescence, the impact of social signals is magnified. When pre-adolescents enter adolescence, they tend to spend more time with their peers, and less time with their parents. They also rely more on their peers than on their parents for guidance and approval. They also show increased attention and higher neural activation in response to peer acceptance. When adolescents feel related to others and when they feel accepted, this is associated with higher self-esteem, and better adjustment in school. In contrast, when adolescents feel rejected, this can lead to school withdrawal, aggression and mental health problems.

Peers impact an adolescent’s behaviour in different ways. For instance, when there are peers in the car, accidents increase. This is only true for adolescents. When there are peers around during a decision-making task, this also leads to higher risky decision-making in adolescents compared to children and adults. Furthermore, when adolescents feel rejected by their peers, they are more likely to engage in risky behaviors, to fit in with the group. One explanation / theory is that feedback from peers serves as a reinforcer, a reward. Research has shown that the neural circuitry that evaluates social (praise, gain, positive affect) rewards overlaps with a neural circuitry that evaluates nonsocial (money, food) rewards. In adults, it has been shown that the ventral striatum (the reward area in the brain) supports learning from social feedback from peers.

In the current article, the goal is to evaluate differences across different age groups in social reinforcement learning from peers, and 8 to 25 year olds will participate. The goal is to determine whether adolescents, compared to children and adults, learn to associate different peers with distinct probabilities of receiving positive feedback. This will be measured with fMRI.

Which methods were used?

Experiment cover story

The experiment consisted of two sessions. In the first session, the participants were lead to believe that they would receive social feedback from peers during a task that they would complete on the second visit. They were shown 5 photographs of gender-, age-, and ethnicity-matched peers. They selected three peers that they would like to interact with. They rated the peers on a scale from 0 to 10 for likeability and attractiveness. Furthermore, participants completed a personal survey where they reported information about themselves. Then, the participants were told that each of the three selected peers would see their survey and the surveys of two others. These peers would write notes on their surveys. Each note that the peers would write would indicate something positive, because the peers had a limited number of notes to write. Notes from peers thus became ‘rewards’.

In the next session, the participants were told that the notes were collected and they would be shown how often the peers decided to write notes to them. However, the delivery of notes was manipulated. Each of the three peers was associated with a distinct probability of social reinforcement, namely ‘rare interaction’ which was defined by 33% of notes and no positive social reinforcement on 66% of the trials. No positive social reinforcement means that the peers decided to write notes for others, but not for the participants. Further, there could be ‘frequent interaction’, which refers to that there was positive social reinforcement on 66% of the trials and no positive social reinforcement on 33% of the trials, and lastly ‘continuous interaction’, which was defined as 100% positive social reinforcement.

What can be concluded?

This experiment showed that there were adolescent-specific age differences in reinforcement learning behavior and neural response patterns. In children and adults, different amounts of positive social reinforcement enhanced learning. In adolescents, all positive social reinforcement lead to lower positive learning rates and higher activity in their response planning circuitry. This means that adolescents do show a unique sensitivity to peers, but adolescent behavior in social contexts is not only explained by simple reinforcement learning theory.

Positive learning rates showed a quadratic patterns, which can mean two things:

  1. Adolescents do not learn to discriminate between cues that are associated with different amounts of positive social feedback, or;
  2. Adolescents’ behavior is not captured by simple reinforcement learning predictions.

Adolescents demonstrate lower positive learning rates, but this does not mean that they do not learn. At the end of the experiment, adolescents as well as children and adults all rate peers who gave them positive feedback as more likeable. There was also no difference in observed negative learning rates across age groups. The adolescent positive learning profile could be explained by an overall vigilance to the receipt of peer approval. This means that a close friend, but also an anonymous or unknown peer can enhance adolescents’ risk-taking behavior. Lower positive learning rates could also be explained by increased motivation toward which is the socially least reinforcing, which is in line with work that suggests that adolescents engage in risky behaviors when they perceive themselves to be less socially accepted. It was also shown that the anterior to mid insula response is correlated with positive prediction error fluctuations and that this is higher than in children and adults. The insula is responsible for processing subjective feelings and awareness about one’s body, feelings of distress, and overall processing of affective states that are the result of interacting with other people. This non-linear finding in the insula supports the hypothesis that peer approval is emotionally salient to adolescents. Furthermore, the data showed that adolescents, more than children and adults, activated regions with response planning circuitry when they received positive social approval, regardless of who the peer was. The data also showed that the ventral striatum and medial prefrontal cortex were equally engaged across age during social reinforcement learning. This means that fundamental reinforcement learning mechanisms support social reinforcement learning from late childhood to adulthood. It seems that the heightened activity in the insular cortex and regions within motor circuitry of adolescents indicates an affective-motivational sensitivity toward any peer approval. There also seem to be differences between different adolescent’s ages; the naturally occurring peak falls in late adolescence. In sum, there is thus an adolescent-specific effect of positive social feedback from peers on learning and neural activation patterns.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of The developing social brain: implications for education by Blakemore - Chapter

Article summary of The developing social brain: implications for education by Blakemore - Chapter

What is discussed in this article?

Humans are extremely social. Studies have shown that social responses are very rapid and instinctive, and that humans have a great ability to read other people’s gestures and facial expressions in terms of their underlying emotions and mental states. Humans are constantly reading other people’s emotional states, which is called ‘theory of mind’ or ‘mentalizing’. This ability to mentalize develops over the first four or five years of life. Around the age of 4, children begin to understand that someone else can hold a belief that differs from their own and which can be false. Mentalizing is extremely important for social interactions.

The importance of social interaction for learning can be described with the following example. Japanese people are unable to distinguish between R and L sounds. However, Japanese babies before the age of nine months are able to distinguish between those sounds. However, when they become older they lose this ability, because they are not exposed to the subtle difference between those sounds. A question is whether this can be relearned. There are two implications from research on this topic. First, relearning is possible. It is possible to relearn sounds after the critical period. Second, social interaction seems to be a critical and constraining factor. There is something special about relearning; when there is a real live person compared to videos, relearning is more present. It is not entirely clear why this effect occurs. It could be that social interaction increases infants’ motivation through enhanced attention and arousal. Social interaction can also help to tailor the training content for the learner. Another important finding is that, by nine months, infants realize that pointing to or looking to an object indicates that this object is being referred to.Studies have determined which brain regions are involved in mentalizing, namely the posterior STS at the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), the temporal poles and the dorsal medial PFC.

In an online mentalizing task, participants view a set of shelves containing objects. They are instructed to move by a ‘Director’, who can see some but not all of the objects. To be able to correctly interpretate the instructions, the participants need to be able to take the director’s perspective, and only move objects that the director can see. This showed that adolescents make more errors than adults, which indicates that the ability to take the perspective of others is still under development in adolescence.

What are implications for education?

Both early childhood and adolescence should be viewed as a sensitive period for learning. The aim of education for adolescents might change to include abilities that are controlled by parts of the brain that undergo most change during adolescence, such as internal control, multitasking, and planning. It might also be helpful to include in the curriculum teaching on the changes that occur in the brain during adolescence. Adolescents might be interested in this, and it might also benefit them to learn how their own brain changes.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Understanding the role of puberty in structural and functional development of the adolescent brain by Goddings et al. - 2019 - Chapter

Article summary of Understanding the role of puberty in structural and functional development of the adolescent brain by Goddings et al. - 2019 - Chapter

Introduction

There is a dramatic increase in publications about the adolescent brain, but there has been less attention given to the role of puberty in the process of brain development in adolescents. Puberty is about the neuroendocrinological development of the adrenal glands, gonads and the growth that leads to reproductive competence. There are also a lot of physical, psychological and social changes. Earlier there was the hypothesis that pubertal development lead to the changes in the brain that occur during adolescence. There was a lack of empirical data to support this hypothesis. Since then there have been studies conducted. This article evaluates the evidence that is found about changes in brain structure, function and connectivity that coincide with pubertal development.

Measuring pubertal development

Puberty comes with adrenarche (activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis) and gonadarche (reactivation of hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis which leads to gonadal activation) which leads to an increase in sex hormones. Age is often used as a determination for pubertal development in animals, but humans show variability in onset (range of 5 years) and tempo of puberty. Therefore it is important to have specific measures. In humans there are two ways of measuring the pubertal development: assessing levels of sex steroid hormones or by assessing objective physical development (like body hair, gonadal development, breast development, menarche).

With the help of phenotypic pubertal assessment scales, the length of exposure to hormones, the levels of exposure and the sensitivity to hormones can be determined. Examples of questionnaires are the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS) and the Tanner Scale. Participants (or parents, teachers, clinicians) answer questions about (their own) pubertal development. When using these questionnaires, the influence of the physical changes during puberty on how adolescents or others perceive themselves, can also be measured. There are also limitations to this kind of assessments, because when there are multiple assessors, there is variability in the answers. Also, the perception people have about their pubertal development is highly influenced by their (social) culture.     

There can also be hormonal measures using serum, saliva or urine to measure the extent of pubertal development. These are useful, because of their objectivity it can help to compare between individuals. There are again, limitations, such as that hormonal concentrations vary from day to day (diurnal variation) and that there are cyclical patterns. They are also influenced by environmental and internal stressors. There is also a lack of understanding of these variations.

Animal evidence for role of puberty and pubertal hormones in the development of the brain

It seems as if puberty is a second sensitive period for sex steroid hormone effects on the brain (the first period being the perinatal period). It could also be that the first sensitive period is until the end of puberty. Research shows that males who are deprived of testosterone during puberty exhibit less developed social and sexual behaviors. Estradiol during adolescence leads to female reproductive behavior in mice and it seems essential for female play behavior in adulthood. These results provide evidence for sex steroid-dependent organizational development and shows that puberty and pubertal hormones play a role in this.

Sex steroid hormone exposure leads to new cell formation and proliferation. Also it seems that these hormones have an influence on cell death: for example, female rats experience greater cell death than male rats during early puberty. So it seems that ovarian hormones may promote cell death in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). A third way in which hormones during puberty influence brain structure and organization is that they influence the complexity and organization of neural dendrites in the brain.

Sex hormones also influence functional brain development. For instance, there have been androgen receptors (AR) and estrogen receptors (ER) found in the brain. These ARs and ERs are found in multiple regions in the brain where they vary in their concentration.

Human studies on the role of puberty and pubertal hormones in the development of the brain

There have been no sex differences found in timing of gray matter development during puberty between men and women, even though this was thought to be true for a long time. Improvements in research methods (specific measures of puberty and longitudinal designs from different cohorts) lead to more reliable findings.

Cortical development

The only published longitudinal study about cortical development indices that pubertal development was related to decreases in gray matter volume. Cortical gray matter contains cortical thickness and surface area.

Subcortical development

It seems that the relationship between puberty and structural brain development is complex and non-linear. It also interacts with the effects of age.

Sex differences in brain structure

The biggest sex difference regarding the brain is in overall volume. This specifically means that boys and men have on average larger brains than girls and women. This could be partially explained by differences in body size. This finding can have implications for research! Also, it seems that boys show greater variability in brain volumes than girls do.

Puberty and Functional Changes in Humans

It seems that during mid-adolescence there is heightened neural activity in the subcortical brain regions that are associated with processing emotions such as rewards, happiness and fear. Some researchers have suggested that this increase is because of heightened sex hormones. These sex hormones may increase the sensitivity in the brain regions. Researchers have also suggested that pubertal development advance social-cognitive processes which rely on social brain network areas. There is some evidence for these hypotheses, but the results are mixed.

Reward processing

When participants won during a gambling task, they showed heightened activity in their ventral striatum and ventral medial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC). These regions are often implicated to be the core of the reward network in the brain. Also, higher levels of testosterone (for both boys and girls) lead to more activity in the ventral striatum. For girls, higher levels of estradiol was correlated with stronger activity in the VMPFC. The latter finding could not be replicated, though the effect of testosterone has been established well over multiple studies. Testosterone also seems to lead to more risk-taking behavior. Estradiol does not seem to significantly relate to risk taking. It even seems as if estradiol leads to less risk-taking!

Processing faces that express emotion

It seems that the amygdala is active when processing emotion on faces, especially when these faces express fear. This neural response in the amygdala when viewing fearful faces peaks during mid-adolescence. But, there has also been found more amygdala activity when viewing happy faces and the results depend on sex. Results have shown that the higher the testosterone levels, the larger the increases in activity in the amygdala and in the ventral striatum when observing fearful faces. This was found for boys as well as for girls. Also, the individuals who showed an increase in ventral striatum activity also showed an increase in activity in the amygdala. This suggests that feelings of fear and of reward are both involved during processing facial expressions.

One study examined how males and females solved incongruent information. This would tell the researchers something about how emotional faces have an impact on thoughts and actions. The study was like this: participants were instructed to approach happy and avoid angry faces, this was the congruent condition. The incongruent condition was to avoid the happy faces and approach the angry faces. It seems that adolescents who had higher testosterone levels, regardless of their sex, showed more activity in the anterior prefrontal cortex for the incongruent condition. Adolescents who had lower levels of testosterone showed stronger activity in the amygdala for incongruent trials. These results show that during pubertal maturation activity from the limbic system (amygdala) shift to more prefrontal control activity (anterior prefrontal cortex).

Processing social cognitive emotions

It seems that pubertal development leads to neural-cognitive development in regions in the brain that are important for the processing of social-cognitive emotions. It is important to note that the literature on neural activity and pubertal changes is mixed: some studies do show that there are neural changes during puberty and others show that this is the other way around. There have not been many studies conducted that link neural development with behavior or that statistically compare sexes. This leads to less directions for future research. But, some directions for further research are to control for menstrual cycle, to include reports from the individual self as from others and to include hormone levels. Also, it is recommended to include boys as well as girls in the studies.

Sex-differences in puberty related neural activity

Reward processing develops in adolescence and there have been sex differences reported. For example, men showed more activation in brain regions for rewards when they could get monetary rewards and women showed greater activation in social-related rewards. During adulthood there were not always sex differences found. This could be because of the nature of the reward or sample sizes. It seems that emotional processing develops in adolescence. There also seems to be evidence for sex differences in adulthood: there seems to be greater activity in the amygdala and gray matter for women than for men and there seems to be greater activity in the insula and prefrontal cortex for men than for women.

In spatial tasks, there is a significant parietal activation for men and women, but women show more frontal lobe activation than men. This could be a compensation for their, on average poorer performance compared to men on spatial tasks. Also, when processing language, men and women use regions in their left hemisphere, but women show some activation in the right hemisphere too.

The role of puberty in brain connectivity

Connectivity in the brain is very important, because deficits lead to neuropsychiatric illnesses (depression, schizophrenia) which often start during puberty. Since sex hormones influence the connectivity, it is important to investigate the relation between the increase of sex hormones during puberty and brain connectivity.

Structural connectivity

The communication between brain regions occurs through axonal pathways that are the base of the structural white matter in the brain. The process of myelination of these pathways continue up to adolescence. The studies that investigate the white matter connections and the pubertal hormones are limited. There is, however, evidence for that estradiol and testosterone are related to the microstructure of white matter. Also, it seems that pubertal development may be related to the maturation of the white matter connectivity.

Functional connectivity

Adults show a more focal pattern in functional connectivity and long-distance connections than children and adolescents do. Children and adolescents show a more diffuse pattern of their functional connections. They also seem to have short-range connectivity. It also seems that during adolescence there is a fine-tuning of the connections between the subcortical and cortical prefrontal and limbic circuits.

It seems that for adults, injecting testosterone leads to disruption in the functional connectivity in their subcortical-cortical functions.

There also seems to be more connectivity in the default mode network (DMN) in women than in men. The DMN involves the posterior cingulate cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex, the angular cortex hippocampus, and precunes. These are the areas that are involved in mentalizing and memory. Men seem to have more connectivity in their visual and dorsal attention networks than women do.

Summary

So, there have not been many studies conducted that study the relation between pubertal hormones and brain connectivity. Recommendations for future research are to focus on the whole brain so that the effects of hormones on the brain can be better understood. This is because all the connections in the brain contribute to a network. Even small changes in parts of this network can have a large effect on the network as a whole. Connectomics is the study of all the connections in the brain. 

Access: 
Public
Article summary of The emergence of depression in adolescence: Development of the prefrontal cortex and the representation of reward by Davey - Chapter

Article summary of The emergence of depression in adolescence: Development of the prefrontal cortex and the representation of reward by Davey - Chapter

Introduction

A great deal of development occurs during adolescence. The adolescence period begins with puberty and ends when the adolescent takes up adult roles and responsibilities. This paper explains how biological development interacts with social development during adolescence. There is an extra focus on the prefrontal cortex. Delay in the maturation of this brain structure compared to the maturation of the limbic areas seems to play an important role in the vulnerability to mental problems.

Depression in adolescence

From the start of puberty, there is a noticeable increase in the number of people who are depressed. The prevalence in childhood is 2 per cent, and 6 per cent at the end of adolescence. Around the age of twenty-five, around 25 per cent of adults has experienced depression at some point in their lives.

Various explanations have been devised for the prevalence of depression during adolescence:

  • Hormonal changes in the brain

  • Physical changes

  • More interpersonal stress

  • Changes in cognitive style and capacity, such as worry behaviour

Recent models

There are three models that try to explain the increased vulnerability to depression during adolescence in an interactive way:

  • Social information processing model. Changes in social behaviour during adolescence are related to the development of social information processing. This includes affection and cognitive regulation. The brain mechanisms that go with this, develop at their own pace. This creates an imbalance, which can lead to extra vulnerability for mental problems. After all, violent responses to emotional stimuli are not inhibited by cognitive regulation.

  • Triadic model. This model consists of three components: approach, evasion and regulation. These three components must explain the changes in adolescent behaviour. The first two components have matured faster than the regulatory component.

  • Dysregulated positive affect. This model looks at the relationship between depression in adolescence and the development of the reward system in the brain. Depression is seen as a shortage of positive affect.

The above models can contribute well to the understanding of strange behaviours in adolescence, such as substance abuse and risky behaviour. However, none of the models explain how the changes in the regulatory and affective systems cause depression. Nor can they explain why part of depression with an onset in adolescence persists in adulthood. The brain systems are already fully matured in adulthood.

The authors of this paper have their own assumptions based on the three aforementioned models. Instead of a mistake in regulation, adolescents have to deal with rewards that are much more abstract in nature than the rewards they have experienced in childhood. This leads to frustration.

The first depressive period

Depression in adolescence is a good predictor of depression in adulthood. Since depression in adolescence resembles often a first depressive period, researchers assume that stress plays a greater role in the development of depression during this period than in later periods. Almost half of the depressed adolescents, for example, experienced a relationship breakup in the year prior to the depression. Rejection by peers also plays an important role.

Reward and depression

A reduced positive affect is often associated with depression in adolescence. This includes symptoms such as energy loss, reduced self-confidence and less appetite. Positive affect is achieved through a rewarding bond with the environment. Interpersonal rewards are particularly important. Connecting rewards show the value of deep interpersonal ties, creating warmth and affection. Agent benefits are related to leadership.

The social risk hypothesis of Allen and Baddock states that symptoms of depression are a reflection of a defensive psychobiological response mechanism when someone perceives interpersonal risks.

The neurobiology of reward

The dopaminergic system is the most important brain mechanism when talking about reward. This concerns the nucleus accumbens and the dopaminergic projections on the ventral tegmental region. Because dopamine is released into the nucleus accumbens, motivation is converted into actions and behaviour is initiated to achieve goals.

Dopamine leads to small changes in basal neuronal activity, but it also reinforces or suppresses responses from other neurotransmitters. For example, glutamate is suppressed.

Serotonin is an important neurotransmitter in the onset of depression. This neurotransmitter communicates with the dopaminergic system to create rewarding feelings. This happens because serotonin is projected on the dopaminergic system by the dorsal raphe nucleus. Serotonin has an inhibitory effect on this system.

Recognizing reward depends on learning. A certain stimulus is seen as rewarding because of the bond with something positive and fine. Reward-related learning is related to changes in the nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal neurons. The hippocampus also plays a role in this, since this structure is related to the storage of memories.

Recent research has shown that the dopaminergic system is activated when one thinks of getting a reward and incentives that predict rewards (and not the reward itself). In addition, the dopaminergic system responds better to new and unexpected rewards. If an expected reward does not come, the dopaminergic system is suppressed.

Depression and reward-based learning

Research shows that depression can be linked to a reduced reward function. However, behavioural studies have been conducted in particular, and few studies with a neurological basis. There appears to be a reduced activity in the ventral striatum with positive stimuli. In addition, there appears to be a reduced activation in the medial prefrontal cortex and increased activation in the orbitofrontal and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex.

EEG studies have shown that activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is related to a stronger bias to respond to reward directions. When less activity can be observed here, this is characteristic of depression.

The dopaminergic system changes as a result of depression; Amphetamines have an enhanced rewarding effect associated with the degree of anhedonia.

Also in the resting state, the reward systems for depressed patients are different. For example, in the amygdala, subgenital anterior cingulate cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex there is an increased activation pattern. The activation pattern in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the hippocampus are lowered.

A few fMRI studies have also been done, but very little has happened.

What we know in any case is that there is a clear link between depression and the reward system. The depressive state is reached as a result of a reduced possibility of finding social reward when the social value of the person concerned is threatened.

Development during adolescence

The development of the adolescent has a major influence on that person in adulthood. For example, how well the adolescent is doing in school, influences the further course of life. Adolescence is an identity-forming period.

Social Experiences

More than a third of their time is spent by the adolescent with peers. Relationships with peers become more complex and hierarchical. In addition, adolescents increasingly deal with large groups with a similar identity. The adolescent's reputation is important for the groups that he or she can join.

Through new motivational systems, sexual development and culture, romantic relationships also develop during adolescence. A change in intercourse is also observed during adolescence. In early adolescence, people in particular deal with people of their own sex. The social groups are gradually becoming more mixed. Next, there is interaction with large groups, ultimately leading to interaction with couples.

Relationships during adolescence are particularly unstable for girls. This also applies to the reputation that adolescents have among their peers. This leads to a relatively high amount of interpersonal stress being experienced in adolescence.

Brain development

Biological changes are the hallmark of adolescence. For example, puberty starts in the brain with the release of large quantities of sex hormones. Recent research has also shown that brain development continues until the mid-twenties. This applies in particular to the prefrontal cortex. Synapse pruning takes place here, there is a reduction in grey matter and greater myelination (so an increase in white matter). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is the last part of the brain that matures.

Due to the increase in myelination, different neuronal networks can communicate better and faster with each other. This is reinforced because synapses are pruned simultaneously, eliminating weak connections.

Changes in the dopaminergic system also occur during adolescence. For example, more dopamine is released in the prefrontal cortex, and the density of the dopaminergic fibres is increased. In early adolescence, dopamine receptors proliferate, which are subsequently pruned, particularly in the subcortical areas (and to a lesser extent, in the prefrontal areas).

The development of the dopamine system during adolescence influences the pruning of compounds in the prefrontal cortical neurons.

Due to the above developments, the dopaminergic system becomes more integrated with the prefrontal cortex. This allows an adolescent to better deal with more complex forms of reward.

Cognitive development

Both the prefrontal cortex and the dopaminergic system are components of the learning process. Both parts of the brain are related to cognitive changes, which means that the adolescent can, for example, think in a more logical and abstract way.

Identity is formed during adolescence. This can be seen in cognitive changes, where adolescents can better apply self-reflection. The adolescent also has an increasingly better image of himself in the past and which direction he or she wants to go in the future.

During adolescence, people also get better at worrying, something that is associated with mental disorders such as depression and anxiety disorders.

The prefrontal cortex

The more complex rewards adolescents anticipate are made possible by the maturing of the prefrontal cortex. This structure is involved in many higher cognitive functions, such as reasoning, planning and abstract thinking.

The prefrontal cortex is relatively larger than that of most other mammals but differs little compared to primates. However, the Broadmann 10 area is significantly larger in humans compared to primates. This area is related to insight into events in the distant future and keeping an eye on long-term goals while completing secondary goals.

The prefrontal cortex is seen as the centre that guides other brain regions in the attainment of goals. This happens because the prefrontal cortex works with rules. These rules are used to activate the desired behaviour to achieve goals.

Another idea is that the prefrontal cortex uses representations of events to determine the desired behaviour in a given situation. This develops during adolescence. Activity in the Broadmann 10 area mediates this.

Suppression of the reward system and depression

The first depressive period during adolescence often arises from the frustration associated with an expected social reward. The expected reward leads to an increased state of excitement. The more abstract rewards can be seen as higher-order rewards, while the more straight-forward rewards are a representation of lower-order rewards. Motivation for the higher order rewards is needed to also be motivated for the lower order rewards. If one is disappointed in a higher order area, this can have an impact on the reward system and thus, in collaboration with other vulnerability factors, a depression can arise.

Time

Because abstract rewards involve an extended sense of time, problems with the reward system due to depression should also entail changes in time perception. People with depression often indicate that they do not feel like the future and that time passes slowly. Research shows that depressed patients overestimate the time it takes. In addition, people with depression are more focused on the past and less on the future.

Individual differences in development and vulnerability

When adolescents are extra sensitive to social situations and when they have difficulty dealing with social disappointments, why doesn't every adolescent develop a depression?

  • Some people have a genetic vulnerability to the development of depression.

  • The social environment plays a role. For example, when someone focuses on multiple long-term goals, disappointment in one goal is less serious than when someone has only one long-term goal.

  • Women are more sensitive to developing depression than men. A possible explanation for this is that in women there are earlier changes in the amount of prefrontal grey matter.

  • The nature of the reward changes in adolescence. The brain cannot always decode it properly.

  • The first period of depression can cause changes that increase individual vulnerability for a subsequent period.

Hypotheses and future research

The prefrontal cortex undergoes major changes in adolescence. Due to the stronger bond with the dopaminergic system and the major social changes during adolescence, this is very important.

It is no coincidence that the increase in the incidence of depression occurs simultaneously with major changes in the prefrontal cortex and an increased sensitivity to social reward. It is not the delayed development of the prefrontal cortex in relation to the limbic areas that seems to be responsible for the development of depression, but the prefrontal cortex in itself.

Future research may focus on patients with a first depressive period and their response to social rewards. The activated areas of the brain should be central here. In addition, it is possible to look at the way in which depressed patients process information and which brain activation can be observed when they have to take various steps to receive a reward.

Access: 
Public
Access: 
Public

Image

Join WorldSupporter!
Search a summary

Image

 

 

Contributions: posts

Help other WorldSupporters with additions, improvements and tips

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.

Image

Spotlight: topics

Image

Check how to use summaries on WorldSupporter.org

Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

How and why use WorldSupporter.org for your summaries and study assistance?

  • For free use of many of the summaries and study aids provided or collected by your fellow students.
  • For free use of many of the lecture and study group notes, exam questions and practice questions.
  • For use of all exclusive summaries and study assistance for those who are member with JoHo WorldSupporter with online access
  • For compiling your own materials and contributions with relevant study help
  • For sharing and finding relevant and interesting summaries, documents, notes, blogs, tips, videos, discussions, activities, recipes, side jobs and more.

Using and finding summaries, notes and practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

  1. Use the summaries home pages for your study or field of study
  2. Use the check and search pages for summaries and study aids by field of study, subject or faculty
  3. Use and follow your (study) organization
    • by using your own student organization as a starting point, and continuing to follow it, easily discover which study materials are relevant to you
    • this option is only available through partner organizations
  4. Check or follow authors or other WorldSupporters
  5. Use the menu above each page to go to the main theme pages for summaries
    • Theme pages can be found for international studies as well as Dutch studies

Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

Quicklinks to fields of study for summaries and study assistance

Main summaries home pages:

Main study fields:

Main study fields NL:

Follow the author: Vintage Supporter
Work for WorldSupporter

Image

JoHo can really use your help!  Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world

Working for JoHo as a student in Leyden

Parttime werken voor JoHo

Statistics
1391