Why do people so often jump to conclusions? - Chapter 7

One of the characteristics of System 1 is jumping to conclusions. Jumping to a conclusion is efficient if the conclusion is likely to be true, the costs of a potential mistake are acceptable and it saves a fair amount of effort and time. It is risky when the stakes are high, the situation is unfamiliar and there is a lack of time for collecting further information. In this case, it is likely to make an intuitive error, unless System 2 intervenes.

If you read a list of letters, including the number ‘13’ in the same shape as the letters, you tend to read it as the letter ‘B’. Would it be the letter ‘B’ in a list of numbers, you tend to read the number ‘13’. This is explained by the fact that the context affects the interpretation of each character. You jump to a conclusion and fail to detect the deviation. When there is no explicit context, System 1 produces a plausible context. When the situation is uncertain, System 1 takes a bet, which is guided by experience. The current context and recent events strongly influence the interpretation. When you do not remember recent events, you rely on older memories (like singing the alphabet). The B/13-example shows that a definite choice was made without you being aware of it. System 1 did not consider alternatives: it does not know conscious doubt. Doubt and uncertainty are typical for System 2.

What is the confirmation bias?

Psychologist Gilbert came up with the theory of believing and unbelieving. He argued that understanding an idea starts with attempting to believe it. What would it mean if it were true? The first attempt to believe is an automatic process of System 1, which constructs the most plausible interpretation of the situation. Even a foolish idea (“birds drink wine”) will initially be believed due to the automatic process of associative memory searching for connections between both ideas that would make sense of it.

Unbelieving is according to Gilbert a process of System 2. When System 2 is engaged, we tend to believe most things. We are more likely to be persuaded by commercials when we are depleted and fatigued.

The operations of associative memory are linked to ‘confirmation bias’. The question ‘Is Naomi nice?’ evokes different memories than the question ‘Is Naomi rude?’. System 2 tests a hypothesis by conscious searching for confirming facts. It is a rule of science to test a hypothesis by trying to refute it, but people (even scientists) tend to search for evidence that supports their beliefs. The confirmation bias of System 1 is guilty of uncritically accepting suggestions and exaggerating the probability of unlikely events. 

What is the halo effect?

If you like someone’s views and opinions, you are likely to also like his/her appearance and voice. The tendency to like or dislike everything about someone, including the unobserved things, is called the ‘halo effect’. This common bias plays a significant role in the way we shape our view of situations and people. It represents the worlds more coherent than it is in reality.

When you meet a man who is approachable and pleasant to speak to, you tend to believe that he is kind to cats. You know nothing about his love for animals, but you like him and you like animal lovers, so by association you believe he likes cats, which makes you even like him more. Evidence was missing, the gap was filled by a guess that matches your feelings. In other cases, your interpretation is influenced by the emotion linked to your first impression. A classic experiment is the following:

Mark: smart – passionate – impulsive – envious – stubborn

Eric: stubborn – envious – impulsive – passionate – smart

Who do you like more? Most people pick Mark, because the first qualities alter the meaning of the qualities mentioned later. The sequence in which we observe things is important, because the halo effect assigns more weight to first impressions.

The halo effect can be tamed by the principle of decorrelate error. Ask a big group of people to estimate the number of marbles in a jar. Individuals will perform poorly, but the whole group usually does well. Some people underestimate the number, others overestimate it, but the average tends to be fairly accurate. The errors that observers make are independent of the errors made by others. Error reduction only works well in case of independent observations and uncorrelated errors. Error reduction fails if the individuals share a bias, as they will influence each other, which leads to a reduced sample size and a less precise group estimation. The most useful data can be derived from multiple sources by making them independent of each other. An example is the rule that multiple witnesses are not allowed to discuss the incident prior to their testimony.

What is the WYSIATI-rule?

Our mind treats currently available information completely different from information that is not retrieved from memory. System 1 construct the most plausible story from currently activated ideas, without considering missing information. The coherence of the created story is it’s measure of success, not the quality and amount of the information it is based on. When there is very little information, which occurs regularly, System 1 jumps to conclusions.

Consider this statement: “Will Carlo be a good boss? He is smart and ambitious.” The answer ‘yes’ immediately popped into your head, based on the limited information available. What if the next words were ‘rude’ and ‘irresponsible’? System 1 only came to the conclusion that being smart and ambitious is positive, which will be revised if novel information becomes available. It does not wait for it though and then there is also the problem of the first-impression-bias.

System 1 seeks for coherence and System 2 is lazy, which indicates that System 2 will support a great amount of intuitive beliefs. Although System 2 is capable of checking the evidence and seeking information that is needed before making a decision, System 1 will still influence these decisions by providing non-stop input.

In order to understand intuitive thinking, you must realize that jumping to conclusions on the basis of very little information is an important part of it. Keep the abbreviation ‘WYSIATI’ in mind (What You See Is All There Is). System 1 is extremely insensitive to the quantity and quality of the information that leads to intuitions and impressions.

An experiment involving jury members who were given one-sided evidence demonstrated the striking effect on their judgments. They also felt more confident in their judgments. The consistency of the evidence mattered more than the completeness of it. The less you know, the easier it is to create a coherent story. WYSIATI induces achieving coherence and cognitive ease, that makes us believe a statement is true. It is why we are fast thinkers and can make sense of incomplete stories. In most cases, the created stories come close to reality and result into appropriate actions. WYSIATI can, however, lead to biases of choice and judgement. Examples are overconfidence (we believe what we see and neglect the possibility that crucial evidence is missing), base-rate neglect and framing effects (presenting the same information in different ways evokes different emotions).

Image

Access: 
Public

Image

Join WorldSupporter!
Check more of the skill?
This content is used in:

Summary of Thinking, Fast and Slow by Kahneman - 1st edition

Search a summary

Image

 

 

Contributions: posts

Help other WorldSupporters with additions, improvements and tips

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.

Image

Spotlight: topics

Check the related and most recent topics and summaries:
Competences and goals for meaningful life:
This content is also used in .....

Image

Check how to use summaries on WorldSupporter.org

Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

How and why use WorldSupporter.org for your summaries and study assistance?

  • For free use of many of the summaries and study aids provided or collected by your fellow students.
  • For free use of many of the lecture and study group notes, exam questions and practice questions.
  • For use of all exclusive summaries and study assistance for those who are member with JoHo WorldSupporter with online access
  • For compiling your own materials and contributions with relevant study help
  • For sharing and finding relevant and interesting summaries, documents, notes, blogs, tips, videos, discussions, activities, recipes, side jobs and more.

Using and finding summaries, notes and practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

  1. Use the summaries home pages for your study or field of study
  2. Use the check and search pages for summaries and study aids by field of study, subject or faculty
  3. Use and follow your (study) organization
    • by using your own student organization as a starting point, and continuing to follow it, easily discover which study materials are relevant to you
    • this option is only available through partner organizations
  4. Check or follow authors or other WorldSupporters
  5. Use the menu above each page to go to the main theme pages for summaries
    • Theme pages can be found for international studies as well as Dutch studies

Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

Quicklinks to fields of study for summaries and study assistance

Main summaries home pages:

Main study fields:

Main study fields NL:

Follow the author: Psychology Supporter
Work for WorldSupporter

Image

JoHo can really use your help!  Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world

Working for JoHo as a student in Leyden

Parttime werken voor JoHo

Statistics
3391