It is not possible to reduce crowd behaviour theories to a generic set of behaviours invariant of the crowd. It is important to focus on collective perceptions of the actors (1), how collective ideologies arouse people’s commitments to action (2) and the importance of actors taking on a collective identity (3). Norm theory states that crowd events are shaped by collective norms emerging in the situation itself. This occurs in a period of milling which precedes action itself. However, it does not explain situations in which crowds act and change quickly without time for milling (1) and according to the theory the norms are formed through group interaction with prominent individuals, making the norms group-based yet individual (2).The self-categorization theory states that defining oneself as a member of a social category is the precondition for group behaviour. This involves a process of self-stereotyping where a person attempts to conform to the standards of the group. The shift from individual to group behaviour involves a shift from personal to social identity. Crowds usually exist in novel, ambiguous and fluid situations in which they are tasked with translating their superordinate social identity into a situational identity without formal means of doing so (e.g. formal hierarchy, debate). It is possible that crowds do this by inferring the nature of group identity from the behaviour of those seen as typical group members The classic agitator theory states that a small group of anti-social individuals are able to take advantage of the fact that crowd members have lost their judgement in order to turn the mass to anti-social ends. In a crowd, there are groups who adopt an ideology of non-violence and will adhere to this even when subject to extreme provocation. Only groups who see violence as potentially legitimate will be open to using violence. The concept of legitimacy determines whether a crowd will enter into conflict under any...


Access options

      How do you get full online access and services on JoHo WorldSupporter.org?

      1 - Go to www JoHo.org, and join JoHo WorldSupporter by choosing a membership + online access
       
      2 - Return to WorldSupporter.org and create an account with the same email address
       
      3 - State your JoHo WorldSupporter Membership during the creation of your account, and you can start using the services
      • You have online access to all free + all exclusive summaries and study notes on WorldSupporter.org and JoHo.org
      • You can use all services on JoHo WorldSupporter.org (EN/NL)
      • You can make use of the tools for work abroad, long journeys, voluntary work, internships and study abroad on JoHo.org (Dutch service)
      Already an account?
      • If you already have a WorldSupporter account than you can change your account status from 'I am not a JoHo WorldSupporter Member' into 'I am a JoHo WorldSupporter Member with full online access
      • Please note: here too you must have used the same email address.
      Are you having trouble logging in or are you having problems logging in?

      Toegangsopties (NL)

      Hoe krijg je volledige toegang en online services op JoHo WorldSupporter.org?

      1 - Ga naar www JoHo.org, en sluit je aan bij JoHo WorldSupporter door een membership met online toegang te kiezen
      2 - Ga terug naar WorldSupporter.org, en maak een account aan met hetzelfde e-mailadres
      3 - Geef bij het account aanmaken je JoHo WorldSupporter membership aan, en je kunt je services direct gebruiken
      • Je hebt nu online toegang tot alle gratis en alle exclusieve samenvattingen en studiehulp op WorldSupporter.org en JoHo.org
      • Je kunt gebruik maken van alle diensten op JoHo WorldSupporter.org (EN/NL)
      • Op JoHo.org kun je gebruik maken van de tools voor werken in het buitenland, verre reizen, vrijwilligerswerk, stages en studeren in het buitenland
      Heb je al een WorldSupporter account?
      • Wanneer je al eerder een WorldSupporter account hebt aangemaakt dan kan je, nadat je bent aangesloten bij JoHo via je 'membership + online access ook je status op WorldSupporter.org aanpassen
      • Je kunt je status aanpassen van 'I am not a JoHo WorldSupporter Member' naar 'I am a JoHo WorldSupporter Member with 'full online access'.
      • Let op: ook hier moet je dan wel hetzelfde email adres gebruikt hebben
      Kom je er niet helemaal uit of heb je problemen met inloggen?

      Join JoHo WorldSupporter!

      What can you choose from?

      JoHo WorldSupporter membership (= from €5 per calendar year):
      • To support the JoHo WorldSupporter and Smokey projects and to contribute to all activities in the field of international cooperation and talent development
      • To use the basic features of JoHo WorldSupporter.org
      JoHo WorldSupporter membership + online access (= from €10 per calendar year):
      • To support the JoHo WorldSupporter and Smokey projects and to contribute to all activities in the field of international cooperation and talent development
      • To use full services on JoHo WorldSupporter.org (EN/NL)
      • For access to the online book summaries and study notes on JoHo.org and Worldsupporter.org
      • To make use of the tools for work abroad, long journeys, voluntary work, internships and study abroad on JoHo.org (NL service)

      Sluit je aan bij JoHo WorldSupporter!  (NL)

      Waar kan je uit kiezen?

      JoHo membership zonder extra services (donateurschap) = €5 per kalenderjaar
      • Voor steun aan de JoHo WorldSupporter en Smokey projecten en een bijdrage aan alle activiteiten op het gebied van internationale samenwerking en talentontwikkeling
      • Voor gebruik van de basisfuncties van JoHo WorldSupporter.org
      • Voor het gebruik van de kortingen en voordelen bij partners
      • Voor gebruik van de voordelen bij verzekeringen en reisverzekeringen zonder assurantiebelasting
      JoHo membership met extra services (abonnee services):  Online toegang Only= €10 per kalenderjaar
      • Voor volledige online toegang en gebruik van alle online boeksamenvattingen en studietools op WorldSupporter.org en JoHo.org
      • voor online toegang tot de tools en services voor werk in het buitenland, lange reizen, vrijwilligerswerk, stages en studie in het buitenland
      • voor online toegang tot de tools en services voor emigratie of lang verblijf in het buitenland
      • voor online toegang tot de tools en services voor competentieverbetering en kwaliteitenonderzoek
      • Voor extra steun aan JoHo, WorldSupporter en Smokey projecten

      Meld je aan, wordt donateur en maak gebruik van de services

      Access: 
      JoHo members

      Image

      This content is also used in .....

      Political Psychology - Article summary [UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM]

      Hammack & Pilecki (2012). Narrative as a root metaphor for Political Psychology - Article summary

      Hammack & Pilecki (2012). Narrative as a root metaphor for Political Psychology - Article summary

      Image

      The narratory principle states that humans, think, perceive, imagine and make moral choices according to narrative structures. This principle can resolve the analytic problem of linking mind and society.

      Narrative refers to the sensible organization of thought through language, internalized or externalized, which serves to create a sense of personal coherence and collective solidarity and to legitimize collective beliefs, emotions and actions.

      Narrative provides access to the current structure of identity, revealing the ideological and experiential content of memory and the motivational anchor for a set of social practices. Narrative engagement refers to the fact that members of a society engage with collective stories of what it means to inhabit a particular political entity (e.g. Dutch). This means that identity is rooted in texts that individuals construct to make sense of their lives.

      Narrative can be defined at two levels:

      1. Proximal level
        Narrative describes a cognitive process of meaning-making and represents an organizing principle for human action. It consists in the mind for the individual.
      2. Multilevel definition of narrative (e.g. history of a nation).
        Narrative reveals the relevance of narrative at the collective level. This consists in the material world for the collective and the individual engages with this narrative.

      Narrative is anchored in four principles:

      1. Language, politics and thought
        The mind is subject to received discourse and the nature of word meanings and storylines affect the way people think about the world.
      2. Personal coherence
        The mind seeks order in time and place and seeks for continuity which can be achieved through story-making.
      3. Meaning in solidarity
        An individual is not a self-contained psychological entity but the need for continuity also exists within the community of shared practice.
      4. Mind in action
        Story-making and narrative engagement are not passive endeavours. Mental processes occur through social practice.

      Meaning of words form concepts and categories which are understood through narrative. Language guides thought and behaviour. The form and content of narratives are arbitrarily constructed as the meaning of the narrative is relative and constituent of a particular way of thinking.

      The discursive approach states that conversation is a mechanism through which individuals create reality and legitimize their positions within it. There are two relevant research methods that make use of this approach:

      1. Political discourse analysis
        This type of research studies the way in which leaders use stories to frame particular political issues and to motivate adherence to a particular political agenda. A weak point of this type of study is that it does not empirically link narrative analyses to individual minds (i.e. the individual responses to these narratives are not known).
      2. Social categories as rhetoric
        This type of research studies how social categorization is a narrative process. It involves the discursive aspects of social categories and its impact.

      How and what people think is rooted in a particular set of meaning-saturated signs and symbols they inherit in a given political setting.

      People tend to construct personal narratives of identity

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy - Article summary

      Van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy - Article summary

      Image

      Group members often do not act on group disadvantage, especially when they perceive themselves as having little efficacy to bring about change. Collective action in response to collective disadvantage might have to do with the degree to which group members perceive the disadvantage as group based and unfair (1), it might have to do with group-based emotions (e.g. anger) (2) or the costs and benefits of action and the perceived efficacy (3).

      The appraisal theory states that appraisal, emotion and action are means by which people cope with events in their social world. There are two types of coping with social reality (e.g. perceived disadvantage):

      1. Problem-focused coping pathway

      This is aimed at changing reality. 

      2. Emotion-focused coping pathway

      This is aimed at regulating the emotions tied to the situation. 

      The problem-focused coping pathway makes use of group efficacy. Collective action might be more likely if group members perceive their disadvantage as unstable. The idea of group efficacy can be strengthened by instrumental social support, the perceived willingness of other group members to take collective action. 

      The emotion-focused coping pathway makes use of emotions tied to the situation. In this case, collective action requires group-based appraisal of unfairness (1) and illegitimacy (2). This evoked group-based emotions of anger which can be strengthened by emotional social support, the support for one’s opinion regarding group disadvantage, and this makes collective action more likely. 

      Collective disadvantage makes the social identity salient. This can lead to group-based appraisal of procedural unfairness in which emotional social support promotes collective action tendencies through group-based anger. It can also lead to group-based appraisal of instrumental social support which strengthens collective action tendencies through group efficacy.

      Collective action requires the perception of a collective disadvantage (1) and a perceived collective identity (2). Collective disadvantage is likely to lead to a salience of social identity. Group efficacy refers to one’s collective belief that group-related problems can be solved by collective effort.

      Group-based anger and group efficacy predict collective action tendencies when one’s in-group is disadvantaged. The highest collective action tendencies are found when there is both emotional social support and instrumental social support. Instrumental social support may enhance perceptions of emotional social support. 

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Wright, Taylor, & Moghaddam (1990). Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective protest. - Article summary

      Wright, Taylor, & Moghaddam (1990). Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective protest. - Article summary

      Image

      There are three limitations to theory and research in intergroup relations:

      1. There is a tendency to focus on feelings and perceptions rather than behaviour.
        There are a lot of theories that attempt to predict strong negative feelings in groups of disadvantaged people. This is not focused on the behaviour as a result of the strong negative feelings.
      2. There is a failure to study a full array of behavioural options.
        There is often only one form of action measured, rather than the full array of behavioural options.
      3. There is a reductionist nature of dominant theories of intergroup relations.
        There is often an extrapolation from intergroup relations to intergroup context.

      A group member engages in collective action anytime that the person acts as a representative of the group and the action is directed at improving the condition of the entire group. Individual action is behaviour that is directed at improving one’s personal condition. Normative actions are actions that conforms to the norms of the existing social structure. Non-normative actions are actions that do not conform to the norms of the existing social structure.

      There are five categories of behaviour:

      1. Apparent acceptance of one’s disadvantaged position.
      2. Attempts at individual upward mobility through normative channels made available by the system.
      3. Individual action outside the norms of the system.
      4. Instigation of collective action within the prescribed norms of the existing system.
      5. Instigation of collective action outside the prescribed norms of the existing system.

      The presence of dissatisfaction or the intensity of negative feelings does not predict the type of action. The presence of alternatives to the disadvantaged situation leads to collective action to improve the position of the disadvantaged group. The absence of alternatives to the disadvantaged situation leads to individual attempts at upward mobility or acceptance of the disadvantaged position. However, it is unclear when group members perceive a situation as unstable or illegitimate, meaning that there are alternatives.

      The five-stage model states that the perception of intergroup situation determines the response of disadvantaged group members. A person will start with individual, normative actions. However, if constant attempts for individual progression are blocked, then the social system will be questioned, accompanied by increased dissatisfaction with the disadvantaged position and more interest in collective action. This model holds that collective action will only be initiated if the advantaged group is perceived as closed.

      People use individual, normative action if the advantaged group is perceived as open. However, this might only be true for individuals who are near meeting the criterion for entrance into the advantaged group. If the advantaged group is partially open, people tend to show little endorsement of collective action and will prefer individual actions.

      People accept their position if the group is perceived as open but they are not near the entry requirement.

      Collective, non-normative action may only be used when

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Deaux, Reid, Martin, & Bikmen (2006). Ideologies of diversity and inequality: Predicting collective action in groups varying in ethnicity and immigrant status - Article summary

      Deaux, Reid, Martin, & Bikmen (2006). Ideologies of diversity and inequality: Predicting collective action in groups varying in ethnicity and immigrant status - Article summary

      Image

      Collective action is one route to affirming the value of one’s group when social comparisons to another group prove unfavourable. Collective action frames are sets of collective beliefs that serve to create a state of mind in which participation in collective action appears meaningful.

      Identification (1) and endorsement of social ideologies that may support or undermine the position of one’s ethnic group in society (2) affect a person’s orientation towards collective action. The strength of identification with one’s group is predictive of willingness to participate in future collective action.

      Concerns for the collective good (1), concerns for social approval (2) and concerns for individual rewards (3) predict collective action. The relative status of a group can shape the actions of its members.

      Social identity theory states that groups that see themselves at a relative disadvantage in comparison with some other group are most likely to act collectively to precipitate change. Groups that are less permeable are more likely to engage in collective action.

      The ideological asymmetry hypothesis (part of the social dominance theory) states that members of high-status groups will be more invested in maintaining current hierarchical relationships than members of low-status groups. Group identification should thus be positively related to the endorsement of hierarchy-enhancing ideologies among members of high-status social groups. This relationship is opposite in people from low-status groups (i.e. they endorse ideologies that do not enhance the hierarchy).

      Multiculturalism refers to an ideological position that supports ethnic diversity and that acknowledges and respects minority group cultures. This means that low-status groups are more likely to endorse multiculturalism.

      The degree to which a person perceives and supports the existence of a status hierarchy within the society influences one’s tendency toward collective action. The support for ideological positions may affect one’s willingness to engage in collective action through its impact on group identification.

      Assimilation refers to the strategy when arriving in a new culture where a person abandons one’s heritage and adopts the host culture. Cultural maintenance refers to a strategy in which members of the group attempt to preserve their culture even as they take actions to enhance their conditions. The cultural maintenance strategy is consistent with the goals of multiculturalism.

      An ideology of diversity could facilitate collective action on behalf of one’s group. Ethnic group identification is positively related to an orientation toward collective action.

      Identification mediates the relationship between ideology and collective action orientation. Social inequality and/or diversity beliefs predicted ethnic group identification and collective action orientation.

      The beliefs about social diversity and social inequality predict collective action orientation and are mediated by identification with ethnic group. This holds in different degrees for different groups:

      1. White natives
        The endorsement of social inequality is positively associated with ethnic group identification. This, in turn, is positively associated with collective action orientation.
      2. White immigrants
        The greater endorsement of social diversity is
      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Klandermans, van der Toorn, & van Stekelenburg (2008). Embeddedness and Identity: How immigrants turn grievances into action. - Article summary

      Klandermans, van der Toorn, & van Stekelenburg (2008). Embeddedness and Identity: How immigrants turn grievances into action. - Article summary

      Image

      The risk of collective action is polarization between groups. Muslim immigrants are expected to assimilate in the host culture which makes it more difficult to use collective action. Social embeddedness might provide individuals with the resources needed to invest in collective action.

      Grievances refer to a sense of indignation about the way authorities are treating a social or political problem. The general assumption of collective action is that people engage in collective action as a response to being aggrieved.

      The relative deprivation theory states that feelings of relative deprivation result from a comparison of one’s situation with a certain standard. There is a distinction between individual relative deprivation and group relative deprivation and group relative deprivation is important for engagement in collective action.

      Distributed justice refers to the fairness of outcome distributions. Procedural justice refers to the fairness of decision-making procedures and the relational aspects of the social process. Procedural justice appears to be more important.

      It appears as if not everyone who is aggrieved engages in collective action while everyone who engages in collective action is aggrieved.

      There are several factors that play a role in whether people who are aggrieved engage in collective action:

      1. Efficacy (i.e. instrumental pathway)
        The availability of resources and the presence of opportunities might play a key role as groups with more resources and opportunities are more likely to succeed in mobilizing collective action. It also helps redress grievances at an affordable cost.
      2. Identity (i.e. identity pathway)
        People engage in collective action to fulfil collective identity needs. The more a person identifies with a group, the more likely that person is to take part in collective action with that group.
      3. Emotions
        Emotions can be approach or avoidance orientated. Emotion is related to efficacy. Fear is associated with feeling less efficacy and anger is associated with feeling more efficacy. Anger is an important stimulant of protest participation and emotions amplify already existing motivations.
      4. Social embeddedness
        This can provide the resources necessary for opposition movements and collective action. Embeddedness in social networks seems to foster conventional and unconventional political participation.

      Dual identity refers to the multiple identities a person can simultaneously have. The identification with a subordinate entity does not necessarily exclude the identification with a supraordinate entity. A dual identity (i.e. integration) might provide more satisfaction with one’s situation than other forms of cultural adaptation.

      Assimilation (1), integration (2), separation (3) and marginalization (4) are four possible forms of cultural adaptation. Integration stimulates sub-group mobilization and thus some form of identification with a nation is needed to mobilize for political action.

      It is possible that people learn how political institutions work by engaging in voluntary associations. Social capital consists of social networks and trust and loyalty.

      Identification with exclusive categories is stronger than identification with inclusive categories. People with dual identities felt more satisfaction about the way

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Reicher (1996). 'The battle of Westminster': Developing the social identity model of crowd behaviour in order to explain the initiation and development of collective conflict. - Article summary

      Reicher (1996). 'The battle of Westminster': Developing the social identity model of crowd behaviour in order to explain the initiation and development of collective conflict. - Article summary

      Image

      It is not possible to reduce crowd behaviour theories to a generic set of behaviours invariant of the crowd. It is important to focus on collective perceptions of the actors (1), how collective ideologies arouse people’s commitments to action (2) and the importance of actors taking on a collective identity (3).

      Norm theory states that crowd events are shaped by collective norms emerging in the situation itself. This occurs in a period of milling which precedes action itself. However, it does not explain situations in which crowds act and change quickly without time for milling (1) and according to the theory the norms are formed through group interaction with prominent individuals, making the norms group-based yet individual (2).

      The self-categorization theory states that defining oneself as a member of a social category is the precondition for group behaviour. This involves a process of self-stereotyping where a person attempts to conform to the standards of the group. The shift from individual to group behaviour involves a shift from personal to social identity.

      Crowds usually exist in novel, ambiguous and fluid situations in which they are tasked with translating their superordinate social identity into a situational identity without formal means of doing so (e.g. formal hierarchy, debate). It is possible that crowds do this by inferring the nature of group identity from the behaviour of those seen as typical group members

      The classic agitator theory states that a small group of anti-social individuals are able to take advantage of the fact that crowd members have lost their judgement in order to turn the mass to anti-social ends.

      In a crowd, there are groups who adopt an ideology of non-violence and will adhere to this even when subject to extreme provocation. Only groups who see violence as potentially legitimate will be open to using violence. The concept of legitimacy determines whether a crowd will enter into conflict under any circumstances (1) and they determine the precise circumstances under which crowds who do not reject violence will enter into conflict (2).

      Conflict only occurs if violence is seen as legitimate (e.g. because of an illegitimate action of the out-group) (1) and when conflict is seen as efficacious (2).

      Crowd members will only enter into conflict with an outgroup where conflictual behaviour is deemed legitimate (1), outgroup action is seen to violate concepts of proper social practice (2) and conflictual tactics are considered an effective way to meet desired ends (3).

      The legitimacy of the conflict (1), the concepts of proper social practice (2), identification of ends (3) and the calculation of whether these ends will be reached (4) are defined with reference to the collective beliefs of the relevant social category.

      Changes in the way crowd members view themselves changes their perceptions of others. Intergroup dynamics are crucial to the onset and development of crowd conflict.

      The limits of involvement in collective conflict are

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Reicher (2016). "La beauté est dans la rue". Four reasons (or perhaps five) to study crowds. - Article summary

      Reicher (2016). "La beauté est dans la rue". Four reasons (or perhaps five) to study crowds. - Article summary

      Image

      In crowds, loss of identity (1), loss of rationality (2) and a loss of morality (3) are thought to occur. Mass society theory states that physical and social divisions between classes makes social order difficult to maintain as people might become vulnerable to manipulation and especially powerful in one single group when freed from a rigid hierarchical order.

      This theory however, attempts to maintain the status quo and sees the crowd member as barbarians who are mindless. This takes away responsibility from the elite in violence of crowds. This theory legitimises repression.

      In politics, crowd are generally seen as unable to properly reason and the collective should be guided and feared. The idea is that individual judgement is always better than collective judgement. This idea might stem from the inherent idea that individual’s reasoning is flawed but extrapolated to the collective.

      In studying crowds, there is a large focus on methodologies which do not reflect the empirical reality and explanations for failed hypotheses will be sought for in methodological flaws rather than empirical flaws. It mostly relies on internal referentiality, the question of how an experiment relates to previous experiments. External referentiality, the question of how an experiment relates to the external world would be more important. The focus of studying crowds is too much on testing hypothesis rather than generating and validating hypotheses.

      Crowd actions are mostly interpreted in the terms and norms of the external observer and when this does not make sense it is put aside as irrational. However, an internal actor might make sense of it and this account can be useful for generating and validating hypotheses.

      The social identity model of crowds states that crowd members shift from personal identity to social identity and do not lose their identity in the crowd. The elaborated social identity model of crowds (ESIM) states that conflict escalates because the outgroup (e.g. police) treats everyone in the ingroup as the same, leading to the notion in the ingroup that the outgroup is a threat and people in the ingroup will feel more connected to one another.

      This theory has three general implications for social psychological theorising:

      1. The theory suggests a more dynamic paradigm for understanding group processes.
      2. The theory suggests that the transformation that are wrought when intergroup dynamics create new forms of common fate and new social identities are more extensive than previously thought.
      3. The theory challenges the dominant perceptualism of social psychology for which understanding and action derive from the ways we look at and process information in the social world.

      The ESIM theory states that people do not make sense of their experience alone but are confronted with multiple influence sources who interpret this experience in different ways. People are mostly influenced by sources who share identity, values and perspective.

      Crowds play an important role in producing the social relations which constitute society. Some

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Feddes, Mann, & Doosje (2015). Increasing self-esteem and empathy to prevent violent radicalization: a longitudinal quantitative evaluation of a resilience training focused on adolescents with a dual identity. - Article summary

      Feddes, Mann, & Doosje (2015). Increasing self-esteem and empathy to prevent violent radicalization: a longitudinal quantitative evaluation of a resilience training focused on adolescents with a dual identity. - Article summary

      Image

      An intervention aimed at empowering individuals in a combination with strengthening empathy is successful in countering violent radicalization. The social identity theory states that people derive part of their self-esteem from the social groups they belong to. People are inclined to perceive their group as better than others which results in positive emotions. However, when people experience relative deprivation, this might lead to negative emotions. This can lead people to distance themselves from society.

      Low self-esteem is associated with aggression. Low self-esteem in adolescence predicts criminal behaviour in adulthood. It is also possible that a high self-esteem with threatened egotism is a strong predictor of violent behaviour. Narcissism might play an important role in mediating these relationships.

      Empathy refers to the capacity to understand and respond to the unique affective experiences of another person. Perspective taking refers to the cognitive ability to anticipate the behaviour and reactions of other people. Perspective taking is related to less egocentric and more other oriented behaviour. Empathy and perspective taking are associated with less prejudice and stereotyping.

      Empathy is negatively correlated with attitudes toward ideology-based violence. Narcissism positively predicts the attitude towards ideology-based violence.

      Narcissism and empathy play an important role in regard to violent radicalization. Self-esteem and agency are not strongly associated with attitude towards ideology-based violence. Narcissism is strongly associated with attitude towards ideology-based violence.

      A moderate level of self-esteem is associated with resilience to violent radicalizations while too high levels of self-esteem can make individuals more susceptible to radicalization. Relative deprivation is a distal factor influencing attitudes toward ideology-based violence.

      Perspective taking is associated with more positive attitudes toward ideology-based violence. This may be due to the low-status group having more benefit from perspective giving whereas a high-status group might benefit more from perspective taking.  

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Heath-Kelly (2012). Counter-terrorism and the counterfactual: Producing the radicalisation discourse and the UK PREVENT strategy. - Article summary

      Heath-Kelly (2012). Counter-terrorism and the counterfactual: Producing the radicalisation discourse and the UK PREVENT strategy. - Article summary

      Image

      Radicalisation refers to the process by which people come to support violent extremism and in some cases join terrorist groups. Risk refers to the processes by which the world is ordered in terms of danger, rather than objective threat.

      The UK PREVENT strategy of terrorist attacks is focused on protection against major disruptive events. There is an emphasis on the value of anticipation and the value of investing in national resilience.

      It is unclear why some people who radicalize resort to violence whilst others do not. Furthermore, it is not clear what is meant by radicalization. There is little empirical evidence for the existence of a radicalization process.

      It is possible that radicalisation knowledge is salient because it produces a possible counterfactual to terrorism by inventing a narrative about transitions to militancy. This allows security mechanisms to perform interventions in the supposed production of terrorism.

      Radicalisation emphasises the individual as driven by persuasive ideology which becomes salient through social networks. Religious terrorist organizations are seen as more dangerous than other terrorist organizations.

      Competing demands of rival cultures sharpen the lines of division and lead to insecurity and rage. This can be part of radicalization. It is often thought that radicalization is ‘contagious’ and that some environments are more ‘ideal’ for radicalization (e.g. prison).

      The British counter-terrorism programmes (e.g. PREVENT) state that the British Muslim population is both at risk and risky. This leads to securitising them concerning what they might produce. There is no distinction between risky and vulnerable.

      Naming a population both risky and at risk blurs the actions of risk-management and securitising governance. This turns vulnerability into potential dangerousness.

      It is unclear when a person is at risk, risky or fully radicalized. The distinction between the groups is unclear. Fear of the unknown tends to create a will to knowledge and when this knowledge is produced, constitutes a governable entity. The self-segregation of Muslim communities is largely seen as problematic.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Pyszczynski et al. (2006). Mortality salience, martyrdom, and military might: The great satan versus the axis of evil - Article summary

      Pyszczynski et al. (2006). Mortality salience, martyrdom, and military might: The great satan versus the axis of evil - Article summary

      Image

      The terror management theory states that people who would normally not condone violent acts on others can be motivated to support acts of aggression and sometimes take up arms themselves when their need for protection from existential fear is heightened and they are being confronted with an outgroup that challenges core aspects of their cultural worldview.

      The potential of existential terror is controlled by maintaining faith in an internalized cultural worldview (1) and obtaining self-esteem by living up to the standards of value prescribed by that worldview (2). The faith in a worldview increases when others share one’s worldview. Other worldviews threaten this faith and this threat is defused by taking action against the other group or disparaging them.

      Mortality salience leads people to conform more closely to the norms of their culture, punish violators of the norm more severely and react more negatively toward those with conflicting world views. Mortality salience increases worldview defence. Mortality salience increases the potential to experience anxiety and this potential motivates worldview defence.

      It is possible that reminders of death increases the willingness of people to support violent action. It is possible that cultural worldviews provide protection from existential fears by providing a view of the world as orderly, predictable, meaningful and permanent. Mortality salience also increases people’s preference for well-structured information. Worldviews that enhance the perceived value of one’s group are likely to be appealing as buffers against existential anxiety.

      The idea of one’s group being engaged in a heroic struggle against evil is a good mean of controlling death-related fear.

      Alienation from the mainstream (1), feelings of humiliation for oneself and the group (2),  desire to avenge past and present grievances (3) and a desire to restore order morality to a world view (4) are important factors in inspiring terrorist violence and support for terrorist violence.

      Thoughts of death appears to lead young people in the Middle East from a generally more preferred pacifist stance to a martyrdom stance which advocates suicide bombings. Mortality salience makes conservatives more likely to endorse extreme military action whereas this is not the case for liberals. Conservatives were more negative of liberals when mortality salience is high but liberals do not change their evaluation of conservatives when mortality salience is high.

      The thoughts of death increases people’s readiness to support extreme violent solutions to global conflicts. Existential concerns about one’s mortality contribute to cultural, ethnic and religious conflicts.

      It is possible that mortality salience effects occur because of a delayed increase in the accessibility of death-related thought and that worldview defences reduce this heightened accessibility back to baseline levels. The function of these defences is to reduce the potential for anxiety endangered by death-related thoughts close to consciousness.

      Reminders of mortality do not amplify tendencies that exist under more neutral conditions but reminders of mortality lead people to gravitate towards conceptions of reality that provide security

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Webber et al. (2018). The road to extremism: Field and experimental evidence that significance loss-induced need for closure fosters radicalization - Article summary

      Webber et al. (2018). The road to extremism: Field and experimental evidence that significance loss-induced need for closure fosters radicalization - Article summary

      Image

      Significance loss occurs in situations that make one feel ashamed, humiliated or demeaned. These situations are inconsistent with the desire for a positive self-image and instil a sense of uncertainty about the self. This leads to motivation for people to seek certainty and closure that affords the restoration of personal significance. This can lead to increased interest in extremist ideologies as they offer a clear-cut strategy for this restoration.

      Circumstances that lower one’s sense of significance induce self-uncertainty. This activates a need for closure. There is a relationship between feelings of insignificance and endorsement of extremism. This relationship may be mediated by a need for closure.

      The significance quest theory (SQT) highlights the role of individual motivation in radicalization. The theory states that extreme behaviour is a mean to gaining or restoring an individual’s sense of personal significance, importance or effectiveness.

      The pathway to radicalization often begins with a triggering event that activates the significance motive (e.g. a situation involving humiliation). There are two forms of significance loss; humiliation directed at one’s social group (1) and humiliation directed at personal circumstances (2). Humiliation directed at a social group is especially powerful for individuals who strongly identify with their group.

      The need for cognitive closure refers to a desire for a quick and decisive answer and an aversion to ambiguity as a result of the circumstances that led to significance loss. Extremism refers to deviancy from a general pattern of behaviour or attitude that prevails in a given social context. Deviancy is relative to a specific standard. Extremism is context dependent.

      Deviancy is typically costly and difficult to sustain. Individuals committed to extreme views tend to protect their deviancy from majority pressures by holding these views with considerable certainty because of the difficulties to sustain extreme views with majority pressures. Extreme views are thus often embedded in confidence-affording and clear-cut ideologies consensually supported by others who share the minority opinion.

      The experience of personal humiliation is positively correlated with a need for closure. The experience of shame is positively correlated with a need for closure.

      Significance loss can lead to extreme attitudes, regardless of participants’ political orientation. Worldview defence refers to increased endorsement of one’s cultural beliefs.

      Extreme beliefs are specifically suited to fulfilling the need for closure induced by feelings of insignificance because it is non-normative. Self-uncertainty increases the appeal of extreme groups. Anxious uncertainty increases extremism.

      The uncertainty-identity theory states that individuals are drawn to more extreme groups because identification with these groups provides a strong mechanism for uncertainty reduction. Extreme groups are adept at reducing uncertainty because they have clear defined group structures (1), a high internal homogeneity (2) and a common goal that has consensus (3).

      Restoration of significance is a good method of rehabilitation of radicalization.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Bar-Tal (2007). Sociopsychological foundations of intractable conflicts. - Article summary

      Bar-Tal (2007). Sociopsychological foundations of intractable conflicts. - Article summary

      Image

      There are several characteristics of an intractable conflict:

      1. Protracted
        The conflict persists for at least a generation.
      2. Violent
        The conflict involves physical violence in which society members are killed and wounded in either wars (1), small-scale military engagements (2) or terrorist attacks (3).
      3. Perceived as irresolvable
        The conflict involves the perception that it is impossible to resolve it peacefully.
      4. Demand extensive investment
        The participants of the conflict make vast material and psychological investments to cope with the situation.
      5. Total
        The conflict is a total conflict. It is perceived as being about essential and basic goals, needs or values that are seen as indispensable for the society’s existence (e.g. the conflict often involves issues of territory, self-determination, statehood, economy, religion or culture).
      6. Perceived as zero-sum in nature
        The conflict does not include compromises and adheres to the original goals. The loss of the other party is seen as a gain.
      7. Central
        The conflict occupies a central place in the lives of the individual society members and the society as a whole.

      The seven features fluctuate in intensity over time. The longevity of the conflict results in both parties accumulating animosity and hostility. The consequences of violence are seen as a problem of society and thus the society takes the responsibility to treat and compensate victims to prevent reoccurrence of the violence.

      Adaptation to the intractable conflict requires satisfying needs that remain deprived during intractable conflict (e.g. need for safety) (1), requires that people learn to cope with stress (2) and requires the development of psychological conditions that will be lead people to attempt to withstand the conflict (3).

      The socio-psychological infrastructure in intractable conflict consists of collective memories (1), ethos of conflict (2) and collective emotional orientation (3).

      Societal beliefs refer to cognitions shared by society members on topics and issues that are of special concern for their society and contribute to their sense of uniqueness. The content of societal beliefs refer to characteristics (1), structures (2) and processes occurring in society (3) and cover different domains of societal life. The societal beliefs provide a basis for common understanding of reality (1), good communication (2), interdependence (3) and the coordination of social activities (4). The societal beliefs constitute the perceived common element in the reality of the society members.

      Collective memory evolves to present the history of the conflict to society members. These memories do not intend to provide an objective history of the past (1) and are shared and treated as a truthful account of the history of the society (2). The collective memories provide a black and white picture which enables parsimonious, fast, unequivocal and simple understanding of the history of the conflict.

      The societal beliefs of collective memory justify the outbreak of the conflict (1), present a positive image of the in-group (2), delegitimize the opponent

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Halperin (2008). Group-based hatred in intractable conflict in Israel. - Article summary

      Halperin (2008). Group-based hatred in intractable conflict in Israel. - Article summary

      Image

      Hatred, anger and fear are central in intractable conflicts. Members of a society involved in an intractable conflict share and repeatedly experience these emotions. These emotions also play an important role in shaping the societal context and in guiding group members’ behaviour in response to conflict events.

      Group-based hatred helps to interpret events (i.e. appraisal) and direct behaviour in a way that contributes to the continuation of the conflict.

      Emotions consist of recognition of the existence of the stimulus (1), an appraisal of the potential effect (2), feelings we have in regard to the stimulus (3) and readiness to take action toward it (4). Emotion refers to an individual’s affective response that develops in reaction to a cognitive appraisal of an environmental stimulus or change and subsequently creates a motivation to take action until there is a return to the balanced relationship between the individual and her environment.

      Group-based hatred is experienced by individuals in the name of their group and as a corollary of their membership in the group. It is directed toward generalized social groups Hatred is a very powerful negative emotion that motivates and may lead to negative behaviours with severe consequences.

      Fear is more dominant in the stage that leads to hatred while anger plays a more central role in the stage of the experience of hatred itself. Common actions as a response to hatred are complete detachment to the object of hatred (1), delight at the expense or failure of the hated other (2) and political action taken against the other (3).

      Hatred refers to a powerful, extreme and persistent emotion that rejects the group toward which it is directed in a generalized and totalistic fashion. Group-based hatred is provoked in consequence to recurrent offenses committed against the individual or one’s group. The offenses are perceived as intentional (1), unjust (2) and threatening (3). Group-based hatred includes cognitive elements that make a clear ideological, moral and cultural differentiation between the in-group and the out-group while delegitimizing the out-group.

      Fear is ruled by the appraisal of low coping potential with future harm. Anger is ruled by the appraisal of out-group actions as non-legitimate or unjust. Hatred is ruled by the appraisal of the harm as intentional and the appraisal of the out-group members having a stable, evil character.

      A perception of high capability to cope with future harm is not an essential precondition to the development of anger in an intractable conflict. In the first stage of the development of group-based hatred, individuals perceive that members of the out-group have caused significant offense to them or members of their group. Anger is developed as individuals from the group conclude that they do not deserve this treatment. Fear is sometimes developed if the future ability to cope with this type of offense is seen as weak. These appraisals, accompanied by the appraisal of intention to harm by the out-group and the

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Mastroianni (2015). Obedience in perspective: Psychology and the holocaust - Article summary

      Mastroianni (2015). Obedience in perspective: Psychology and the holocaust - Article summary

      Image

      Milgram’s explanation of obedience for perpetrator behaviour in the holocaust is currently being challenged. The situationists state that the situation leads to a particular behaviour (e.g. Milgram’s experiment; Stanford prison experiment).

      It is likely that atrocities (e.g. holocaust) do not merely occur because of the situational factors, although situational factors do play a role. Good leadership is likely to play an important role in these events.

      It is possible that the situationist explanation of atrocities is readily accepted due to a lack of detailed knowledge about these events. This lack of understanding can be explained by the public understanding being shaped by an incomplete and sometimes inaccurate account (1), historical scholarship has continued to refine out understanding of the events (2) and Milgram stated that historical context should be ignored (3).

      According to Waller, genocide is possible because of our ancestral shadow (1), the identities of the perpetrators (2), a culture of cruelty (3) and social death of the victims (4). This theory relies on biological and social mechanisms that tend to universalize the potential for genocidal behaviour and downplay contextual historical factors.

      According to Baumeister, genocide is explained by idealism (1), threatened egotism (2), instrumentalism (3) and sadism (4). Idealism and threatened egotism are the primary factors relevant to the explanation of the Holocaust.

      According to Staub, genocide starts with difficult life conditions. These conditions frustrate basic human needs and attempts by groups or individuals to explain or address these difficult conditions can operate to promote intergroup hostility and violence. Cultural devaluation (1), authority orientation (2), an aggressive past (3) and the lingering effects of past victimization (4) as risk factors of genocide.

      Invalidation of individual volition (e.g. in situationist explanations) in genocidal behaviour (i.e. voluntary actions) renders moral judgement of behaviour problematic (1) and it deflects attention from the beliefs and ideas that play an important role in genocide (2).

      It is likely that a lot of genocidal behaviour can be explained and understood as a consequence of normal mechanisms of perception, learning, socialization and development.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Strauss (2007). What is the relationship between hate radio and violence? Rethinking Rwanda's 'radio machete'. - Article summary

      Strauss (2007). What is the relationship between hate radio and violence? Rethinking Rwanda's 'radio machete'. - Article summary

      Image

      It is often thought that during the Rwandan genocide, the radio conditioned, facilitated and legitimized violence and became a tool for the mobilization of genocide. It is also often thought that the radio was a voice of authority.

      The claim that the radio indirectly instilled ideas that led to violence and issued orders that directly led to mobilization have three primary weaknesses:

      1. The claims are incompatible with mainstream political communication research.
      2. The literature on Rwanda imply a simplistic and improbable model of agency.
      3. The discussions of media violence are often not situated in a broader discussion of the dynamics of violence or of an assessment of rival explanation.

      The claims of the effect of the radio on the genocide in Rwanda reflect the hypodermic needle theory. There is often no attribution of agency to the listeners of the radio. The dynamics of violence are often simplified in the account that the radio is accountable for the genocide.

      However, it is not clear what the effects of the radio were exactly and it is possible that the radio did not reach all areas where violence against Tutsis occurred. The onset of violence in Rwanda was not simultaneously.

      The broadcast range of the radio cannot account for the time of onset of the violence, meaning that the radio cannot be the sole trigger of the genocide. It might have been the case that the radio served to bolster and encourage those who were committing violence.

      Perpetrators often claim that the radio was not the primary cause of joining in actions to commit genocide. Face-to-face mobilization and social ties were the primary factors through which ordinary citizens joined the genocide. However, it is possible that the radio catalysed the more hard core among the broader pool of perpetrators. The radio, most likely, had conditional and marginal effects.

      The war in Rwanda led to legitimization of the killings (1) and created a sense of uncertainty and fear (2). This led some people to become radicalized and led others to believe killing was necessary.

      The media effects were probably catalysing hard-liners (1), reinforcing messages (2) and framing public choice (3).

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Cehajic, Brown, & Castano (2008). Forgive and forget? Antecedents and consequences of intergroup forgiveness in Bosnia and Herzegovina. - Article summary

      Cehajic, Brown, & Castano (2008). Forgive and forget? Antecedents and consequences of intergroup forgiveness in Bosnia and Herzegovina. - Article summary

      Image

      Forgiveness refers to the prosocial orientation whose aim is the restoration of a relationship with someone who has committed some offence to the victim. Forgiveness is associated with approach-tendencies and the motivation to pursue relationship-constructive actions is mediated by empathy for the offender. A victim’s decision to forgive is associated with the release of feelings of anger and revenge towards the offender.

      Intergroup forgiveness refers to a reduction of feelings of revenge, anger and mistrust towards the perpetrator group and intentions to understand, approach and engage with its members.

      The contact hypothesis states that intergroup contact is a key variable in improving intergroup relations. A willingness to forgive a perpetrator requires a belief that the perpetrator will not exploit such a move as a sign of weakness. Forgiveness thus requires trust. Trust facilitates forgiveness and contact increases trust. Empathy improves outgroup attitudes. Empathy mediates the effects of contact on intergroup forgiveness.

      Contact with the outgroup may facilitate forgiveness of the group through subgrouping or reducing the outgroup homogeneity effect. Frequent and high-quality intergroup contact might facilitate intergroup forgiveness by enhancing trust of the outgroup (1), the amount of perspective taking (i.e. a form of empathy) (2) and the perception of outgroup heterogeneity (3).

      Social identity theory (SIT) states that there is a direct link between people’s identification with the ingroup and the tendency to positively differentiate the ingroup from outgroups in terms of judgements, attitudes and behaviour. The common ingroup identity model states that if intergroup encounters can be engineered to foster a more inclusive categorisation of the situation such that the ingroup and outgroup become subsumed into a single enlarged ingroup (e.g. humans vs. ethnic group), intergroup relations would benefit. This leads to the notion that former outgroup members are now seen as ingroup.

      In order to facilitate forgiveness, people do not have to give up their sub-group identification but need to identify more strongly with the superordinate group.

      Positive intergroup contact and high common-ingroup identification led to greater forgiveness. This was associated with lessened social distance from the outgroup. Intergroup contact impacted on forgiveness through empathy for and trust in the outgroup and the perception of outgroup variability.

      An unwillingness to forgive the outgroup might be due to a resistance to forget the past as forgiving is often associated with forgetting.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Hornsey & Wohl (2013). We are sorry: Intergroup apologies and their tenuous link with intergroup forgiveness. - Article summary

      Hornsey & Wohl (2013). We are sorry: Intergroup apologies and their tenuous link with intergroup forgiveness. - Article summary

      Image

      Intergroup apologies have increased significantly since the second world war. A failure to acknowledge transgressions of the past can breed intergroup conflict in the future. High levels of collective guilt promote greater intentions to compensate for an historical transgression.

      For the victim, intergroup apologies represent an acknowledgement of past harm (1), an affirmation of shared moral standards (2), a recalibration of power and status relationships (3), a bestowment of dignity (4), a symbolic gesture of intent for the future (5) and a chance to move forward (6).

      For perpetrators, intergroup apologies represent an opportunity to unshackle the collective memory from guilt (1), a removal of a moral stain (2) and an investment in reconciliation and intergroup harmony in the future (3).

      Apologies refer to communications in which offending parties acknowledge responsibility or guilt for an offence and express regret or remorse for this offence to an offended party. It requires the expression of responsibility. Apologies often contain promises of forbearances and offer of repair. Forgiveness refers to the belief that it involves a change in emotion and attitude away from the negative states regarding the offender towards more positive states. Forgiveness of the conflict does not equal forgetting the conflict.

      People are more willing to forgive transgressions committed by their own group members than by similar transgressions attributed to the outgroup. Seeing the perpetrator as part of a common, inclusive social category (e.g. humans) promotes forgiveness.

      Apologising appears to lead to a reduced desire for retribution and increased forgiveness. The desire for retribution is mediated by anger. Forgiveness is mediated by respect. Generally, people seem to be more willing to forgive when there is an apology. However, some research shows that there is no evidence for an apology-forgiveness link.

      There is research that suggests that there is potential for interpersonal apologies to produce interpersonal forgiveness but intergroup apologies have no observable effect on intergroup forgiveness.

      Process models emphasise the temporal evolution of forgiveness. Apologies and other gestures of reconciliation plant a seed of forgiveness that may bloom after a period of emotional and cognitive processing. People’s representation of apology histories appeared to be largely unfitting with reality.

      Historical victimhood may offer a rallying point for solidarity and defending the group from accusations of moral transgression. The more people believe that an apology is sincerely offered, the greater their expectations that the apology would improve intergroup relations. The link between an apology and forgiveness appears to weaken over time. It is possible that this occurs because expectations are not being met.

      The offer of compensation with an apology is seen as insulting but less so when the apology is based on shame rather than guilt.

      Primary emotions refer to primitive, basic emotions experienced by both humans and other animals (e.g. fear, anger). Secondary emotions refer to higher-order emotions, presumed to be experienced only by humans (e.g. shame, guilt). The capacity to experience secondary

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Hornsey et al. (2017). Conservatives are more reluctant to give and receive apologies than liberals - Article summary

      Hornsey et al. (2017). Conservatives are more reluctant to give and receive apologies than liberals - Article summary

      Image

      People are much more likely to forgive when the transgressor had apologized. Apologies help heal relationships that have been threatened by a breach of trust.

      Apologies include a statement of transformation (i.e. the transgressor has changed). People who hold entity theories (i.e. the self is not prone to change) are less likely to give an apology and are less forgiving. People who hold relatively conservative values are more likely to hold entity beliefs about human nature.

      The exchange theory states that transgressions lead to a disequilibrium in the relationship. Apologies restore equity in the relationship, leading to reduced need for additional punishment. The needs-based model of reconciliation states that transgressors and victims have different goals that they seek to achieve through the reconciliation process. The transgressor’s primary motive is to restore their moral image in the eyes of others. The victim’s primary motive is to restore power, control, honour and status.

      Social dominance orientation (SDO) assesses people’s general orientation to the existence of societal hierarchies. People low in SDO see equality as a virtuous goal whereas people high in SDO see power differences as normal, natural and unproblematic. People who hold relatively conservative values are more likely to be high in SDO.

      The more conservative people are, the more they endorse both SDO and entity beliefs which resulted in a reluctance to apologize.

      The more conservative people are, the less likely they are to use apologetic statements. Conservatives are also less influenced by apologies when determining whether a transgressor deserved forgiveness. There is no clear-cut evidence that entity beliefs are related to the willingness to provide and reward apologies.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Rimé et al. (2011). The impact of gacaca tribunals in Rwanda: Psychosocial effects of participation in a truth and reconciliation process after a genocide." "Cho (2013). Campaign tone, political affect and communicative engagement. - Article summary

      Rimé et al. (2011). The impact of gacaca tribunals in Rwanda: Psychosocial effects of participation in a truth and reconciliation process after a genocide." "Cho (2013). Campaign tone, political affect and communicative engagement. - Article summary

      Image

      A truth and reconciliation committee (TRC) are temporary non-judicial institutions where perpetrators and victims can express themselves in order to increase understanding and empathy after a conflict (e.g. civil war, genocide).

      Durkheim’s model of collective rituals states that collective rituals involve both at a time group symbols (e.g. flags) and collective expression (e.g. singing). The simultaneous activation of shared beliefs and emotions enhances perceived similarity among participants. The more an event provokes an emotion, the more it elicits social sharing. The coordinated interaction and reciprocal stimulation enhances empathy and emotional contagion. A climate of emotional communion takes place by boosting reciprocal attraction. The participant’s shared beliefs are put to the foreground and the self becomes less salient. This would lead individuals to leave the collective situation with enhanced self-confidence and renewed faith in life.

      There is no good evidence that TRCs heal. Increases in negative antagonist emotions are strongly related to empowerment effects from participation in TRC for victims. TRCs can be seen as expiatory rituals that aim at restoring harmony and social cohesion by reasserting a community’s norms and values.

      According to the self-categorization theory (SCT), social identification results from a cognitive self-categorization process due to the particular salience of social categories in the collective situation.

      The need of victims is recovering some level of control and power. The need of perpetrators is regaining a positive image. Reconciliation attitudes will be reinforced if victims receive a message of empowerment and perpetrators receive a message of social acceptance. The victim’s self-esteem also needs to be increased.

      Victims will be empowered by receiving information (1), apologies (2), reparation (3) and having the possibility to accept or reject the apology (4). Perpetrators will be able to erase past negative facets of their self by manifesting pro-social behaviours (1), apologizing (2) and accepting punishment (3). The perpetrators accept shame whereas the victims reduce shame and increase pride in their social identity.

      Participation in a transitional ritual (e.g. TRC) reactivated negative emotions in both victims and perpetrators. Participation in Gacaca increased negative resignation emotions (1) and improved social integration (2).

      Two consequences of participation to a collective ritual are triggering a strong reactivation of the emotions associated with the commemorated event (1) and contributing to the reconstruction of participants’ collective identity by boosting group cohesion and social integration (2).

      The mere expression of emotions in a collective situation does not have healing effects.

      The Gacaca had several effects:

      • It strengthened participants’ self-definition in non-ethnic terms.
      • It increased out-group perceived heterogeneity.
      • It led to participants expressing more positive stereotypes of outgroup members.
      • It led victims (but not perpetrators) to perceive the emotional climate as more positive and involving more cohesion.
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Cho (2013). Campaign tone, political affect and communicative engagement. - Article summary

      Cho (2013). Campaign tone, political affect and communicative engagement. - Article summary

      Image

      Political advertising encourages people to engage in communication activities during election cycles. Political discussion is a stable, positive predictor of political participation. There are four types of advertisement tone:

      1. Attack ads about the candidate the voter favours.
      2. Attack ads about the candidate the voter opposes.
      3. Advocacy ads about the candidate the voter favours.
      4. Advocacy ads about the candidate the voter opposes.

      The disposition and surveillance system explain affective responses to external stimuli and outcome consequences. The disposition system’s function is to monitor habits and manage on how we rely on what has previously been learned. This system is activated when a new stimulus is as expected and signals safety. It allows people to rely on habits and routines in processing information.

      The surveillance system’s function is to identify novel or threatening situations and drives conscious attention to and reasoned consideration of the situations by elevating anxiety levels. This system is activated when information is novel or challenges expectations. The surveillance system results in more effortful processing and a conscious behavioural strategy that keeps people alert.

      It is possible that these systems guide emotional experiences and subsequent behaviours during political ads. The affective intelligence theory states that political stimuli that challenge voters’ values and beliefs endanger symbolic worlds and produce results that are similar to more dangerous stimuli from non-political contexts.

      Attack ads often employ the guilt by association strategy and use highly emotional language. Exposure to attack ads is positively related with feelings of anxiety and anger that a voter feels towards the candidate. However, attack ads can also create a boomerang effect because many people disapprove of the sponsor of attack advertising.

      The black-sheep effect states that evaluations of attack ads and the sponsors of the ads are driven by a motivation to preserve a positive social identity. Therefore, when people see an in-group candidate engaging in unfavourable behaviour, this is perceived as violating group norms and threatening the positive image of the group. This leads voters to evaluate the candidate negatively. If voters do not perceive an in-group candidate’s attack message as placing group identity in danger, there is less motivation to distance themselves from the candidate. Issue-based attack ads are seen as more acceptable than personal attack ads.

      Advocacy ads regarding the preferred candidate leads to quick and easy information processing and the voter’s emotions will reflect the positivity of the promotional ads.

      Attacking ads regarding the opposed candidate leads to quick and easy information processing and voters are less motivated to thoroughly review the criticisms because of the congruent nature of the message. Voters will probably follow the ads and find reasons to maintain or intensify their prior attitudes. Exposure to attacking ads about the opposed candidate will strengthen the pre-existing negative attitudes towards the candidate.

      Advocacy ads regarding the opposed candidate leads to anxiety but not anger for the voter. This is because anxiety

      .....read more
      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Marcus, MacKuen, & Neuman (2011). Parsimony and complexity: Developing and testing theories of affective intelligence. - Article summary

      Marcus, MacKuen, & Neuman (2011). Parsimony and complexity: Developing and testing theories of affective intelligence. - Article summary

      Image

      The theory of affective intelligence states that an individual’s emotions help govern a reliance on political habits or govern deliberation and attention to new political information. A dispositional system of emotion incorporates people’s normal feelings about things. Dispositions are guided by enthusiasm and aversion.

      A surveillance system of emotion manages attention. It relies on anxiety, fear or uncertainty. It requires emotions that notify that conscious attention is necessary. Partisanship becomes less powerful when the surveillance system is active. Citizens stimulated by anxiety will vote in accord with their sense of which candidate promises the best policy package.

      A one-dimensional conceptualization of affect misses much about human experience. There are three dimensions of emotion that have distinctive effects on political attention:

      1. Enthusiasm
      2. Aversion
      3. Anxiety

      Enthusiasm drives active participation. Aversion pushes avoidance and the defensive reactions of motivated reasoning. Anxiety triggers attention and learning.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Lecheler, Schuck, & de Vreese (2013). Dealing with feelings: Positive and negative discrete emotions as mediators of news framing effects. - Article summary

      Lecheler, Schuck, & de Vreese (2013). Dealing with feelings: Positive and negative discrete emotions as mediators of news framing effects. - Article summary

      Image

      Emotions are a powerful force within the political communication process. Emotions are integral components of news frames and exposure to news framing can cause an individual to have specific emotional reactions. Some frames can cause a stronger emotional response than others.

      News frames are patterns of interpretation that are used to classify information sensibly and process it efficiently. It stresses certain aspects of reality (i.e. a selective function). It suggests certain attributes, judgements and decisions. Equivalency frames refer to logically alike content which is presented or phrased differently. Emphasis frames refer to qualitatively different yet potentially relevant considerations. Issue-specific frames refer to frames regarding a specific topic. Generic news frames refer to frames regarding a wide variety of topics.

      Emotions refer to internal mental states representing evaluative reactions to events, agents or objects that vary in intensity. They are generally short-lived, intense and directed at external stimuli.

      The appraisal theory states that the development of an emotional state depends on individual and subjective evaluations of that event. This means that a cognitive appraisal of a specific event precedes emotional response to a news frame.

      Discrete negative emotions (e.g. anxiety; anger) affect political attitudes and behaviour negatively. Explicitly positive emotions (e.g. enthusiasm, hope) affect political attitudes and participation positively. Message-induced anger leads to more attention and more careful information processing. Message-induced fear leads to a low willingness to process information.

      Enthusiasm is related to mobilizing action tendencies and deep-information processing. Contentment is associated with immobility and the failure to process a message. Positive news framing contributes to higher levels of support for the political issue at stake. Enthusiasm is a mediator of news framing on political opinion. Anger is a mediator of news framing on political opinion. Only anger and enthusiasm affect opinions while anger, enthusiasm, fear and contentment are affected by news framing.

      News frames cause emotional responses which led to higher or lower support for a political issue. Anger and enthusiasm mediate the framing effect. Frame strength depends on whether a frame causes emotional reactions and which emotional response this frame triggers.

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Stolwijk, Schuck, & de Vreese (2016). How anxiety and enthusiasm help explain the bandwagon effect. - Article summary

      Stolwijk, Schuck, & de Vreese (2016). How anxiety and enthusiasm help explain the bandwagon effect. - Article summary

      Image

      The bandwagon effect refers to voters increasing their support for the ‘winner’ in election polls. The underdog effect refers to voters increasing their support for the ‘loser’ in election polls.

      Emotional excitement (1), enthusiasm of the crowd (2) and defection to avoid disappointment (3) are causes for the bandwagon effect. Media messages may evoke emotions which mediate the effects these messages have on attitudes.

      The bandwagon effect consists of three effects:

      1. Voters previously intending to vote for a losing party will be more likely to change their intention and vote for a winning party.
      2. Voters intending to vote for a winning party will less likely to change their intention.
      3. The bandwagon effect could occur for undecided voters who crystallize their intention due to poll exposure.

      The bandwagon effect refers to a voter’s increased likelihood to vote for a party after exposure to more positive poll coverage about that party. The journalist interpretation of polls may be important for the bandwagon effect. The amount of polls a person is exposed to is also important for the bandwagon effect. The more polls are reported in a media outlet and the more frequent an individual uses this outlet, the higher the chances are for this individual to observe these polls.

      Enthusiasm reinforces existing attitudes and beliefs. Anxiety leads to reconsideration of previously held beliefs which makes voting for the initially favoured party less likely. It might also be that vote choice is altered as a result of unexpected enthusiasm and anxiety (i.e. expectancy violation theory). This means that unexpected emotions make a vote switch more likely.

      The effect of poll exposure on vote choice is positive. The effect of poll exposure on enthusiasm and party ratings is positive. The effect of enthusiasm and party ratings on vote choice is positive.

      The effect of anxiety on party ratings and vote choice is negative. Emotions do not explain the bandwagon effect entirely.

      Anxiety and enthusiasm are mediators between poll exposure and vote choice. If aversion does not play a specific and prominent role in an election campaign, then consecutive events are likely to trigger multiple negative emotions about a loathed party rather than just aversion. Emotions might become blended over time and could occur consecutively.

      The influence of polls on vote choice relies more on the descriptions of journalists than on the bare poll figures by themselves. The amount of exposure to poll evaluations in the media contributes to individuals changing their vote intention over the course of the electoral campaign.

       

      Access: 
      JoHo members
      Political Psychology - Summary [UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM]

      Political Psychology - Summary [UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM]

      Image

      This bundle contains everything you need to know for the course "Political Psychology" at the University of Amsterdam. It contains the lectures. In the lectures, the following articles are discussed:

      "Hammack & Pilecki (2012). Narrative as a root metaphor for Political Psychology".
      "Van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy."
      "Wright, Taylor, & Moghaddam (1990). Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective protest."
      "Deax, Reid, Martin, & Bikmen (2006). Ideologies of diversity and inequality: Predicting collective

      .......read more
      Follow the author: JesperN
      Comments, Compliments & Kudos:

      Add new contribution

      CAPTCHA
      This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
      Image CAPTCHA
      Enter the characters shown in the image.
      Promotions
      vacatures

      JoHo kan jouw hulp goed gebruiken! Check hier de diverse studentenbanen die aansluiten bij je studie, je competenties verbeteren, je cv versterken en een bijdrage leveren aan een tolerantere wereld

      Check how to use summaries on WorldSupporter.org


      Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

      Using and finding summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

      There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

      1. Starting Pages: for some fields of study and some university curricula editors have created (start) magazines where customised selections of summaries are put together to smoothen navigation. When you have found a magazine of your likings, add that page to your favorites so you can easily go to that starting point directly from your profile during future visits. Below you will find some start magazines per field of study
      2. Use the menu above every page to go to one of the main starting pages
      3. Tags & Taxonomy: gives you insight in the amount of summaries that are tagged by authors on specific subjects. This type of navigation can help find summaries that you could have missed when just using the search tools. Tags are organised per field of study and per study institution. Note: not all content is tagged thoroughly, so when this approach doesn't give the results you were looking for, please check the search tool as back up
      4. Follow authors or (study) organizations: by following individual users, authors and your study organizations you are likely to discover more relevant study materials.
      5. Search tool : 'quick & dirty'- not very elegant but the fastest way to find a specific summary of a book or study assistance with a specific course or subject. The search tool is also available at the bottom of most pages

      Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

      Quicklinks to fields of study (main tags and taxonomy terms)

      Field of study

      Check related topics:
      Activities abroad, studies and working fields
      Institutions and organizations
      Access level of this page
      • Public
      • WorldSupporters only
      • JoHo members
      • Private
      Statistics
      1475