Summaries per article with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University 22/23

Summaries per article with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University 22/23

Check supporting content in full:
Article summary of Emotions by Scherer - Chapter

Article summary of Emotions by Scherer - Chapter


Preface

An emotion consists of various components, namely physical arousal, motor expressions, action tendencies and subjective feelings. These components have an effect on social cognitions, attitudes and social interactions. That is why it is important that emotions are signaled during a conversation. This is about the emotion that is being emitted, so the non-verbal communication. Emotions play a role in forming and breaking social relationships. People also like to talk to others about emotions. One of the most important areas of social psychology where emotions are important is within group dynamics. This concerns the effects of 'contagious emotions'; passing a felt emotion over to others. Examples of contagious emotions are laughter and yawning.

What is an emotion?

The James-Lange theory

The James-Lange theory takes a peripheral position (which means that it focuses more on the somatic and autonomous rather than the central nervous system). In addition, it is suggested that someone first perceives an event, after which a physical reaction occurs. Then, only after the sensation of that physical reaction, an emotion occours. The difference between the James-Lange theory and the theories before, was that in the James-Lange theory it was thought that an emotion would come only after the physical reaction and in the theories before the main idea was that that an emotion would come before a physical reaction.

Emotion as a social-psychological construct

Nowadays there is a growing consensus that 'emotion' should not be used as a synonym for 'feeling'. Instead, researchers suggest that feelings are one of the three components in the emotion construct. Other components are the neurophysiological responses and motor expressions. These 3 components together are called the 'emotional reaction triad'. Another component that belongs to this emotion construct is the action tendency, although this is also seen as a behavioral consequence rather than a component of emotion. In addition, the emotion construct includes a cognitive component, because there is always evaluative information processing when it comes to emotion-generating events. The cognitive interpretation of an event is also called an appraisal. An emotion is described as a fierce, dynamic and short process with a clear beginning and an end. This involves as a crisis response, in which the physiological and psychological components interact with each other during an emotion episode. Systems that were previously independent suddenly start working together in synchronization to ensure survival.

Why do we have emotions?

Emotions cost a lot of energy, so why do they exist?

The evolutionary significance of emotions

According to Darwin, emotions exist because they are adaptive and help regulate interactions within social living species (for example, raising eyebrows provides better vision).

Emotions as a social signaling system

Another explanation for the existence of emotions is that, because one person can express emotions, another person can respond to this more easily and this can also lead to a certain tendency towards action.

Emotions provide behavioral flexibility

Emotions are almost automatic, but are more flexible than normal stimulus-response responses. Emotions ensure 'latency time' between stimulus and action, which ensures that people are better at evaluating the situation. During that period, the chance of success and the seriousness of the consequences are examined, after which an optimal response can be chosen. If there is a negative consequence, the motivation to take action will be great. Therefore, emotions have a strong influence on motivation.

Information processing

Information processing which is done people, especially in the social field, usually consists of 'hot cognition'. These are emotional responses that help to evaluate relevant and irrelevant stimuli. The criteria used in the evaluation of stimuli are learned during conversations and are influenced by needs, preferences, goals and values.

Regulation and control

Our feelings are a constant monitor of what is happening, and thus serve as the evaluation and appraisal of the environment, physical changes in the central nervous system and action tendencies. This is a requirement when controlling or manipulating the emotion process.

So, an emotion:

  • decouples stimulus and response
  • ensures the (correct) action trends through a 'latency time'
  • provides signals for the outside world (others)
  • feelings can regulate emotional behav, which can be strateic in social interactions

How are emotions elicited and how are they distinguished?

Philosophical notions

It is clear to most philosophers that a certain situation is reacted with a certain type of emotion.

The Schachter-Singer theory of emotion

According to Schachter, two factors are important in eliciting and distinguishing emotions, namely the perception of arousal and cognitions. Arousal is always the same (non-specific) and cognition leads to a label of the emotion (for example fear). In an experiment, arousal was generated in participants by means of an adrenaline injection. This showed that cognitions labeled this arousal for events that were taking place in their environment at that time. Emotions are thus formed by felt arousal and by the cognitive interpretation of the situations that are based on the behavioral model of expression. The results have not been replicated.

Appraisal theory

The appraisal theory of Lazarus consists of primary appraisal (fun / dislike, helps / hinders achievement of the goal) and secondary appraisal (to what extent can the person deal with the consequences of an event, given his or her competences, resources and strength). Lazarus calls this model a transactional model, because the outcome of the event is not only influenced by the nature of the event, but also by the needs, goals and resources of the person. It is different for each person and often leads to a mix of emotions (emotion blend).

Cultural and individual differences in appraisal at events

Culture causes differences in appraisal, for example socialism versus individualism. In a collectivist culture, guilt and shame are seen as the result of immoral things. In an individualistic culture this only applies to guilt and this emotion also lasts longer than in collectivist cultures. So the socio-cultural value can influence someone's emotional life. Individual differences in appraisal also cause different emotional responses.

Are there specific response patterns for different types of emotions?

There is agreement about the differentiation of the emotional component of emotions, but not about the reaction patterns of the peripheral system. James uses proprioceptive feedback (sensory information from organs about physical changes) to differentiate between emotions. Schachter and Singer, on the other hand, believe that non-specific physiological arousal combined with situational factors ensure that emotions can be differentiated. Tomkins spoke about discrete emotions, where he talked about neural programs that can control a certain emotion and the associated facial expression and motor skills.

Wat are motoric expressions?

Facial expressions

Evidence has shown found that facial expressions are universal, even though small differences have been found between cultures due to cultural desirability (display rules). 

Vocal expressions

Emotions are not only recognizable by facial expressions, but also by vocal expressions. Here too there are differences between people and cultures. Emotions in voice are partly universal, even though there are language differences between cultures. This is proof of a partial biological basis of emotions.

Control and strategic manipulation of an expression

Cultural norms about appropriate expression of an emotion are called display rules. It concerns the regulation of 'congenital' systems. In addition to the fact that it is appropriate to control your emotion expression because of cultural norms, it is also important from a strategic point of view. This would allow someone to manipulate someone else. Emotion expression often only comes into being when we see other people and that is why it is seen as a communication tool. But the more an emotion overwhelms us, the harder it is to regulate it.

Physiological changes

Physiological activity is not communicative, but it provides energy. This can ensure that someone is prepared for a specific action. Studies show specific patterns for the emotions fear and anger. These are functional: in case of fear, blood flows to the heart and brain to prevent blood loss. In the case of anger, the blood flows to the muscles for action.

Subjective feelings

This involves someone's conscious experience about the processes that take place in his or her body.

Dimensions of feeling

Wundt made a three-dimensional system to display the precise nature of all complex emotional states. The three dimensions are: excitement - depression, tension - relaxation, pleasant - unpleasant. There is only evidence for the first and third dimensions and therefore, in other studies, they often use a two-dimensional model of emotions. 

Verbal labeling of feelings

Emotions are socially structured (which means that the social and cultural factors create a reality for an individual). Cultural differences in value judgment systems, social structures, communication habits and other factors influence the emotion experiences and are reflected in culturally specified states of feeling. Feelings that are verbally expressed are influenced more quickly by sociocultural variations than other components of the emotion process. This makes sense because the subjective state of feeling represents the cultural and situational context and the other components of the emotion process.

How can emotion components interact?

Research has shown that the components of emotions are all strongly interconnected.

Catharsis

Catharsis revolves around the interaction of three components of emotion, namely expression, physiology and feeling. Through an expression, a person can calm himself down, reduce his arousal and at the same time change his state of feeling.

Proprioceptive feedback

Proprioceptive feedback (or the facial feedback hypothesis) states the opposite of the catharsis hypothesis. In this case, inhibition of facial expression reduces the intensity of an emotion and emphatic facial expressions can enhance the intensity of an emotion. In an experiment, participants had to hold a pen between their lips or teeth. The participants who used their laughing muscles to hold their pen rated the cartoons they saw as funnier. The effects were even stronger when the participants saw themselves in the mirror and the effects were also stronger with participants with high self-awareness. This has the opposite effect when someone has to smile kindly, while the person is actually furious, because this only reinforces the anger.

Access: 
Public
Articlesummary with Anger response styles in Chinese and Dutch children: A sociocultural perspective on anger regulation by Novin a.o. - 2011
Article summary of Emotion awareness in children with high-functioning autism by Rieffe et al. - Chapter

Article summary of Emotion awareness in children with high-functioning autism by Rieffe et al. - Chapter


Introduction

Children with autism experience difficulties when moving around others. It is also suggested that they do not have a properly functioning Theory of Mind (ToM). This ToM is needed to be aware of your own mental states and to understand your own functioning. Children with autism who do not have a properly functioning ToM will not be able to evaluate themselves.

How do autistic children show emotional awareness?

Children with autism appear to show more emotions when viewing non-living objects compared to children without this disorder. This is striking, because they react less emotionally in social situations. They do not make sufficient or even no use of the communicative values ​​of emotional expressions. There is no further reason to believe that they are less emotional than others. An emotional state does not directly involve emotional awareness. Controlling one's own emotional processes is a capacity that children must learn.

To label emotions, one needs introspective information, his or her own behavioral observations, and the verbal information provided by others. Normally developing children do not yet have a good introspective capacity. Due to the lack of self-reflection, they cannot properly explain what they feel. Adults have better introspection and are better able to explain how they feel and why. However, it is hardly possible to explain what it is that they feel.

According to cognitive emotion theorists, we become aware of a primitive action tendency. For example, fear comes to the fore due to the tendency to run away. The question is whether is the same in children with autism who do not have a fully functioning ToM. Earlier research has shown that autistic children understand the link between the four basic emotions (fear, anger, joy and sorrow) and their causal events. However, only through introspection or through the careful observation of others will anyone be able to find out that there are always exceptions to the rules. Children with autism are known to ignore the emotional expressions of others. Especially in exceptional situations, an austistic child will not notice the emotion. For example, fear is linked to a large dog. If someone is then afraid of small downy hamsters, a child with autism will not notice the fear of the person in question. Research has shown that they are able to give descriptions and give examples of various emotions, but their examples refer less often to social interactions.

Multiple emotions

It is important to note that in addition to a few prototypical situations, almost all situations can be viewed from different perspectives.
When young children have discovered one of the perspectives, they tend to stop analyzing. Around nine years old, children generally begin to recognize the existence of simultaneous emotion perspectives. First they discover that it is possible to have two similar emotions at the same time. These are emotions of the same valence (two positive or two negative), for example angry and sad. A little later they discover that it is possible to feel two equal emotions of different valence at the same time, such as happy and sad.

The possibility of looking at one and the same situation from different perspectives is a possibility that is also closely related to the Theory of Mind. To have a Theory of Mind, it is necessary to understand that someone can have a different perspective on things. Different emotions in the same situation also reflect different action trends.

What are the objectives of this study?

The first goal of this study is to investigate the ability of high functioning children with autism to produce concrete examples of emotion-provoking situations from their own experiences. As a hypothesis it was expected that there will be no differences between normally developing children and children with autism in terms of their ability of possible situations in which they experience one of the basic emotions. They could therefore come up with about as many possible situations. The reactions of the children were analyzed in two ways. First, the extent to which the children use their own experiences as a frame of reference was studied. Then it was studied whether the examples are social or non-social examples.

The second goal was to study whether highly functioning children with autism are aware of the multiple emotional impressions of situations that contain different emotional perspectives. The hypothesis was that children with autism would score worse than normally developed children.

Which research method was applied?

Participants

22 children with autism (20 boys, 2 girls) and 22 normally developing children (20 boys, 2 girls). The average age was 10 years and 2 months. The autistic children were all diagnosed with Classic Autism or Asperger's syndrome according to the DSM-IV criteria.

Emotion identification

As an introduction, the children were asked to identify 'pride'. The emotion served as a useful introductory question, which also provided a better balance between negative and positive emotions. The four basic emotions (fear, anger, joy, and sadness) that followed to identify were presented in a random order. To see to what extent the children recognized their own emotional experiences, the children were asked questions.

Scoring

The reactions of the children were classified as social (explicit references to another person) or non-social. In addition, references to a specific situation or event were interpreted as an indication that children used their own experiences as a frame of reference, rather than using prescribed knowledge about emotion-provoking events.

Multiple emotions

To start with, the children were asked to imagine that the event in the story happened to them. The Multiple Emotions task consists of an example story (accompanied by drawings) and the instruction to inform the children about the possibility of experiencing more than one emotion at a time. Children viewed drawings of social expressions, which correspond to the four emotions in the story. They were asked what emotions the child would feel and how intense these emotions are. Afterwards, four stories were offered to the children. Two of these stories were designed to provoke both positive and negative emotions (joy and sadness). The other two stories were designed to provoke multiple negative emotions (anger and sadness). To avoid too many stories with a negative impression, two stories were added that were designed to only provoke joy.

What are the results?

Some emotions

In comparison with the control group, children with autism were more likely to deny that they had ever experienced one or more of the four emotions when asked about some emotions. In addition, they reported fewer negative emotions. These results were only significant for anger. The children with autism made fewer references to specific situations and gave fewer social examples than the group with normally developed children. The intensity of the emotions showed that joy was experienced more strongly than the three negative emotions. This was the same for the experimental and control group.

Multiple emotions

Among the multiple emotions, it turned out that autistic children named fewer different emotional perspectives per story. This result does not directly suggest that these children recognize multiple emotional perspectives less often, because it is possible to attribute more than two emotional perspectives to the same multiple emotion scenario. Furthermore, it was found that children from both autistic and normally developing groups more often recognized the multiple perspective when it involved two negative emotions. The average intensity experienced with the emotions joy and anxiety also did not differ between the two groups. Finally, it turned out that if the autistic children recognized the other two emotions (anger and sadness), they reported these emotions as being less intense. 

Discussion

Children with autism would feel less to no emotions, they could report fewer emotional situations from their own experience and recognize fewer different emotional perspectives in the multiple emotion scenarios. Children with autism are similar to the control group when detecting emotions from an opposite domain in a scenario. They are not the same when detecting emotions from the same domain. This suggests that we cannot attribute their problems to delays in development. The recognition of simultaneous emotions from the same domain normally precedes the recognition of the simultaneous appearance of opposing emotions. The research also shows that autistic children find it difficult to identify their own emotions. This means that it is difficult for them to state how they feel. The experimental group of children would not have a well-developed knowledge of their own emotions, which can be indicated by the finding that they also use fewer coping strategies to deal with the negative emotions. The results of this study seem to point to a simpler, single-emotion perspective in the negative domain with a more important position of anxiety in children with autism than in the control group, rather than using multiple emotion perspectives.

Access: 
Public
Articlesummary with Verbal display rule knowledge: A cultural and developmental perspective by Matthew Wice a.o. - 2019
Articlesummary with The roles of shame and guilt in the development of aggression in adolescents with and without hearing loss by Broekhof a.o. - 2021
Articlesummary with Longitudinal effects of emotion awareness and regulation on mental health symptoms in adolescents with and without hearing loss by Adva Eichengreen a.o. - 2022
Articlesummary with "My child will actually say 'I am upset'... Before all they would do was scream": Teaching parents emotion validation in a social care setting by Lambie a.o. - 2020
Articlesummary with Moral emotions and moral behavior by Tangney a.o. - 2007
Article summary of The emotional development of children with regards to relationships with others by Von Salisch - Chapter

Article summary of The emotional development of children with regards to relationships with others by Von Salisch - Chapter


Introduction

Within a few years, children show incredible growth in their emotional development. They learn to deal with different emotional situations between their fifth and twelfth year of life. In the past, most research has viewed this emotional growth as single or intrapsychic processes. Few studies have taken a transactional or interpersonal perspective when it comes to emotional developments while all people are born as social beings who cannot survive or develop without significant relationships with other people. There are several theories that emphasize the importance of an interpersonal perspective on emotions. However, most of these theories focus on one or a few components of emotions, namely the development of cognition, subjective experiences or the expression of emotions. It is important to view these separately because these components each follow a different and independent development path.

Current research looks at theoretical formulations and empirical findings with regard to emotional development in the three interpersonal relationships that are most important to primary school children. These are their relationship with their parents, their peers and their friends. The purpose of this review is to show the challenges that children face within emotional development in relation to these individuals. Challenges are tasks that confront children in their emotional life in a certain relationship at a certain moment of their development. These emotional challenges can cause problems and difficulties, but can lead to development and adaptation in the long term.

Emotional development: parents and children

Previous theories emphasize the importance of fathers and mothers on the emotional development of their children through the reciprocal investment that takes place over a longer period of time. This relationship is asymmetrical because for a long time the parent has more to say in the interaction than the child. Two important aspects of the relationship between children and their parents are the support that children receive and the learning that occurs.

For a long time, parents are the primary caregivers to children when they experience pain, anxiety or stress. They help children with their emotion regulation. Until adolescence, and sometimes longer, parents play a major role in the psychological functioning of their children at difficult moments. Parents teach their children basic lessons on how to regulate emotions. This is influenced by, for example, the responsiveness of parents and the form of attachment of the children to the parents. Parents are also the emotional coaches of children. By talking about feelings, they learn their child how to regulate these emotions. They also transfer culturally prescribed and valued rules with regard to experience and showing emotions (display rules). A limitation on the help that parents can offer is that they are further developed cognitively and emotionally than their children. For example, if their children are afraid of a ghost, this will not scare the parent. In addition, they will sometimes limit children in their emotions, for example if their emotions are culturally inappropriate at a certain moment. The extent to which parents influence their children and the emotion regulation and the way in which, differs per parent and depends on individual differences between parents. Individual parents differ in their willingness and capacity to respond empathically to their children if they show signs of stress.

Emotional development: peers

The relationship between children and their parents is symmetrical because both partners have about the same amount of social power. Relationships with peers, such as classmates, are often involuntary and many are not close, meaning that they do not share intimate thoughts or activities with each other. Yet peers seem to have an important influence on the emotional development of children. First of all, peers seem to be in a better position to understand each other's emotional lives than parents or children of other ages by the same age. Secondly, peers form a group. Being together with a group can reinforce some children's emotions, such as laughter at school and joint activities and or fears. As a group, children and adolescents create a culture with their own norms and values.

In groups of peers, the norm now seems to be to reduce the expression of emotions in many situations. Empirical research shows that children indicate that they only report anxiety and pain to their peers if it occurred to an extreme extent or if it was visible from the outside, such as a wound or bleeding. Primary school children expect more negative reactions if they show fear or sadness in their peers than in their parents. These rules appear to occur not only in fear and sadness but also in anger. This standard appears to be particularly strict with boys.

There are two possible ways in which peers can ensure that a culture is created and preserved in which the expression of emotions is muted. One method is teasing and bullying, a method that occurs in children who go to school. The other mechanism is more indirect: through gossip. Peers tend to reject children who do not fit their rules about showing emotions.

Emotional development: friendship

Friendship is often only distinguished from relationships with 'normal peers' in the pre-adolescence phase, because friendship then reaches a new level of intimacy. A difference between friends and peers is that friends choose each other. Friendships are therefore voluntary relationships that are generally based on mutual sympathy. Friends who are close can help each other to see which feelings are 'appropriate' and which are not. They also learn what expectations go with a friendship, such as 'being there for your friend when he or she needs you'. In addition, close friends also learn how to deal with disagreements. Also, children learn to deal with feelings of competition in friendship. Friendships are vulnerable because we voluntarily choose our friends. Anger, jealousy and other negative emotions can cause a friendship to change or end.

Conclusion

Many of the studies have been conducted in Western industrialized countries. This limits the generalizability of the results. A further limitation is that many studies have used self-reporting. These self-reports are not objective and the reports may be biased because, for example, social desirability. In the last ten years there has been a large number of studies on emotional development. It is now time to delve deeper into these studies and the material studied. It is time to make a distinction between the various components of emotions and to look into the development of attention and physiological components of emotions in future research.

Access: 
Public
Articlesummary with Caring babies: Concern for others in distress during infancy by Dadidov a.o. - 2020
Articlesummary with Reactive/proactive aggression and affective/cognitive empathy in children with ASD by Pouw a.o. - 2013
Articlesummary with The developmental trajectory of empathy and its association with early symptoms of psychopathology in children with and without hearing loss by Tsou a.o. - 2021
Article summary of Altered neurobiological processing of unintentional social norm violations: a multiplex, multigenerational functional magnetic resonance imaging study on social anxiety endophenotypes by Janna Marie Bas-Hoogendam et al. - Chapter

Article summary of Altered neurobiological processing of unintentional social norm violations: a multiplex, multigenerational functional magnetic resonance imaging study on social anxiety endophenotypes by Janna Marie Bas-Hoogendam et al. - Chapter


What is the background of this study?

People with SAD (social anxiety disorder) fear being negatively evaluated in social settings. The characteristics of SAD include an onset during early adolescence, often a lifelong course and a great risk of comorbid disorders. While SAD has severe effects on one’s personal life and society, current treatment is not optimal. In order to improve interventions, the risk factors of developing SAD need to become more clear.

A patient with SAD fears acting in a way that will be humiliating or embarrassing in social situations. The behavioral and neurobiological correlates of that fear can be examined by using the Social Norm Processing Task. This task involves reading and evaluating stories describing (un)intentional violations of social norms. Studies using this Task found that people with social anxiety are more sensitive to unintentional violations of social norms. One study showed increased activation in the medial prefrontal cortex in the participants with SAD. In comparison to the control group, they also considered the stories more embarrassing and inappropriate, especially the story describing unintentional violations. Thus, social anxiety affects the embarrassment ratings for unintentional violations of social norms. Another study showed that participants with greater social anxiety levels rated the unintentional violations just as embarrassing as the intentional violations. Experiencing embarrassment underlies the development and maintenance of social anxiety.

However, no study examined whether the correlates of altered processing of unintentional social norm violations could be SAD endophenotypes. Endophenotypes are heritable and measurable components indicative of genetic vulnerability to disorders. These endophenotypes could improve our knowledge regarding the pathways leading to severe psychopathology, help identifying vulnerable individuals and improve the current intervention programs. Endophenotypes need to be related to the disorder, independent and already present in a preclinical state, heritable and they should cosegregate with the disorder in families of probands.

Hypothesis: brain activation and behavioral ratings are associated with processing unintentional violations of social norms.

Who participated and what materials were used?

Participants were recruited through the LFLSAD: Leiden Family Lab study on Social Anxiety Disorder (a multigenerational and multiplex family study). Eight families participated, including two generations of families carrying the gene linked to SAD. The inclusion of the families was based on the combination of a ‘proband’ parent (diagnosed with SAD) and a child who met criteria for SAD. Used were MRI scans and various measurements such as diagnostic interviews.

The Revised Social Norm Processing Task (R-SNPT) consisted of story reading, getting into the MRI scanner and rating the MRI scan. The stories described a neutral social situation and a situation in which a social norm was unintentionally or intentionally violated (baking a pie with friends: using the right amount of sugar, accidentally using salt, using salt as a prank). Participants had to imagine themselves in these social situations. After the MRI scan, they were asked to rate the stories’ inappropriateness and embarrassment.

What were the results?

No gender and age differences were observed between family members with and without SAD. Family members with SAD displayed higher levels of social anxiety.

Self-reported social anxiety was positively linked to brain activation in two brain clusters. One cluster was located in the occipital pole, the other in the frontal pole. No clusters showed negative associations with social anxiety. Heritability assessments suggest that brain activation in those clusters is heritable.

The R-SNPT ratings showed a significant relation between embarrassment and social anxiety, but none between inappropriateness and social anxiety. Follow-up analyses suggest positive associations between social anxiety and embarrassment regarding all three stories. Sensitivity analyses suggest that these findings were not driven by SAD. Embarrassment ratings in the intentional stories showed low heritability. Embarrassment scores for neutral and unintentional stories showed no heritability.

What is the conclusion of this study?

This study proves that brain activation associated with processing unintentional violations of social norms is a neurobiological candidate endophenotype of social anxiety disorder. These findings provide new insights in the neurological pathways leading to social anxiety disorder.

Access: 
Public
Article summary of Social emotions and social cognition in the development of social anxiety disorder by M. Nikolic et al. - Chapter

Article summary of Social emotions and social cognition in the development of social anxiety disorder by M. Nikolic et al. - Chapter


Humans have a social nature: they value others’ opinions and share norms of the groups they belong to. Some people are so extremely and often worried about others’ opinion of themselves that it highly affects their social life. They suffer from social anxiety disorder (SAD), a very common disorder in Western countries. Approximately 1 in 10 people receive this diagnosis. SAD is related to loneliness, low quality of life, societal costs and comorbidity with other mental issues. It has an early onset (average age 10-13 years), but the first symptoms of social anxiety can emerge earlier (preschool age). Once developed, SAD often has a lifelong course while treatment outcomes are poor. This emphasizes the relevance of examining the factors influencing the development of SAD. A better understanding of these risk factors may help improving current treatments.

Dysregulated emotions are seen as a symptom of SAD, but they may also play a key role in the development of SAD. The feelings experienced by children in social situations may influence their behavior in those situations, for instance avoiding the situation. Avoiding social situations leads to social anxiety and the development of SAD. A disturbed social cognition (how children think about others and themselves in social situations) may also be of relevance.

What is the role of social emotions in the development of SAD?

Social emotions are the emotions that emerge in social situations: in relation to social norms or other people. These emotions are disturbed in individuals with SAD: they are excessively afraid of others’ opinions and are highly self-conscious. So far, it has been not clear whether these dysregulated emotions are a risk factor for SAD. This study is aimed at examining whether shyness and fear exist in young children with elevated levels of social anxiety and whether they influence the development of SAD.

How is social fear related to social anxiety?

Social fear is the fear of potentially being negatively evaluated by others in social situations, for example when being the centre of attention or during interaction with strangers. Fear is mostly shown through bodily and facial signs. Responding with fearful behaviors is also known as ‘behavioral inhibition’, which includes avoidance and withdrawal in social situations. Previous research indicates that behavioral inhibition in infants and young children is a predictor of the onset of SAD in late childhood and adolescence. However, it is unknown when and how behavioral inhibition influences the development of SAD. This knowledge could be helpful in the prevention and treatment of SAD.

In this study, social behavioral inhibition in social situations was examined in young children aged between 2.5 and 4.5 years. One situation involved a conversation with a stranger. Early social anxiety symptoms were measured by using parent reports, SAD symptoms in children aged 7.5 years were diagnosed by a clinician and reported by parents. It was found that social behavioral inhibition at the age of 2.5 and 4.5 years is a predictor of early onset of social anxiety symptoms at 7.5 years. Conclusion: social behavioral inhibition during toddler- and childhood is a risk factor for a later SAD diagnosis.

From a physiological perspective, fear often goes together with hyperarousal, since it activates the sympathetic system (measured through EDA: electrodermal activity) and withdrawals the parasympathetic autonomic nervous system (measured through HR: heart rate and HF-HRV: high-frequency heart rate variability). In this study, it was examined whether physiological hyperarousal in social situations is related to elevated social anxiety levels in early childhood (before a SAD-diagnosis), by measuring the participating children’s EDA, HR and HRV while singing a song in front of others. The findings show that physiological hyperarousal (reduced HRV and increased sweating) was related to higher social anxiety levels.

It was also examined whether physiological hyperarousal influences the development of SAD symptoms. The findings show that children exhibiting reduced HRV and increased HR during their interaction with a stranger displayed more SAD symptoms at a later age.

How is shyness related to social anxiety?

Shyness is an expression of self-consciousness. Although shy children want to interact, they are anxious and afraid of rejection. Shyness can be exhibited through positive shy expressions (approach-dominant) or through negative shy expressions (avoidant-dominant). A child displaying numerous negative shy expressions is considered more socially anxious, due to avoiding and withdrawing from social situations.

In this study, the relation between negative and positive shyness in social situations and social anxiety was examined. The findings show that negative shy expressions were positively associated with social anxiety symptoms, while positive shy expressions were negatively associated with social anxiety symptoms. How children exhibit their shyness can thus be predictive of their development.

From a physiological perspective, shyness is often exhibited through blushing. Individuals with SAD are considered to blush more often and easily in social situations. The findings of this study prove that individuals with SAD blush more in social settings. In children aged 4.5 years, more blushing was associated with more social anxiety symptoms. Also, blushing was found predictive of the development of SAD symptoms at the age of 7.5-12 years. There are two possible explanations. Being self-conscious about blushing may result into distress and avoidance and eventually into SAD. Another explication might be that blushing is the result of experiencing social stress due to continuous social exposure, which leads to the avoidance of future social situations and increased social anxiety.

What is the role of social cognition in the development of SAD?

Previous research showed that impairments in socio-cognitive abilities are related to elevated social anxiety levels in children aged 4-11 years. The findings of this study indicate a negative association between social anxiety and ToM (Theory of Mind, the capacity to comprehend and predict someone else’s behavior based on their emotions) in children aged 4.5 years. Also, impairments in understanding emotions are associated with elevated social anxiety levels.

Previous studies found high or low levels of socio-cognitive abilities in social anxiety. It is possible that both levels are associated with SAD symptoms in children. This notion was analysed in this study through mindreading: detecting one’s mental state through facial expressions. Children with SAD often express rejection or disapproval. The findings show that both high and low levels of mindreading are associated with elevated social anxiety symptoms in children aged 8-12.

What are the conclusions?

The results of this study indicate two possible pathways to the development of SAD: one through elevated self-consciousness and one through fearful temperament. Fearful responses in social situations are especially important in early childhood (2.5-4.5 years), because it could be predictive of developing SAD at a later moment in life.

Dysregulated social emotions and elevated levels of socio-cognitive skills may be risk factors for SAD, so the treatment of SAD during childhood should focus on socio-cognitive abilities and emotional experiences. For instance, task concentration training trains children to concentrate on the task rather than on themselves.

Access: 
Public
Articlesummary with Affective empathy, cognitive empathy and social attention in children at high risk of criminal behaviour by Zonneveld a.o. - 2017
Articlesummary with How biosocial research can improve interventions for antisocial behavior by Glenn & McCauley - 2018
Articlesummary with Children who are deaf or hard of hearing in inclusive educational settings: a literature review on interactions with peers by Xie a.o. - 2014
Check page access:
Public
Check more or recent content:

Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology: Summaries, Study Notes and Practice Exams - UL

Summaries per article with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University 22/23

Summaries per article with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University 22/23

Summaries per article with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University 22/23

Supporting content: 
Access: 
Public
Lecture notes with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University - 2015/2016

Lecture notes with Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology at Leiden University - 2015/2016


Lecture 1: What are emotions and why do we have them?

Emotions in folk psychology

There is a huge difference between scientific knowledge and ‘kitchen’ psychology. One of the folk psychology ideas is that you should be rational, and not act upon your emotions. Another common idea in the folk psychology is that ‘my heart says one thing, but my head says something else’, and that ‘emotions come over me, they happen to me’ or ‘time heals’.

Emotions: helpful or harmful?

Emotions are very important signals, just like pain tells you that something needs to be looked after in your body. Emotions tell you that something meaningful has happened. Everything else disappears, emotions give you a very clear focus. That is also what you communicate with your environment. The worst thing you can do is to ignore your emotions, just like it’s bad to ignore your pain, because it will only get worse when you neglect it.

Functional emotion theory

Frijda (1986) is one of the founding fathers of the current emotion theory. It is still very valid today. He says that there’s always a concern at stake. Something meaningful is happening, it can be either a positive or a negative emotion. These emotions, positive or negative, cause changes in action readiness. You can either fight or flight, but in both situations the whole body is involved. This is called physical arousal, which prepares the body to react to the situation. It is aimed to change or maintain relationships. This is the core of the whole emotion theory: emotions are there because you want or don’t want something in relation to others. It’s all about relationships with other persons. We have emotions to regulate our relationships with others. Sadness is a very powerful emotion, because it brings the focus of the person who sees that you’re sad on you. So the goal of this emotion is to get attention from someone, so you can get help from that person. There are competing theories, but the functionalistic perspective is the most important perspective. This is the perspective of Frijda.

James-Lange theory (1884/5)

James-Lange has a different view on emotions. First there is the perception of an event. After that there is physiological arousal, and because we feel this physiological arousal, we’re having a subjective feeling. Thus, emotion is our feeling (awareness) of the bodily changes as they.....read more

Access: 
Public
College-aantekeningen bij Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology aan de Universiteit Leiden - 2015/2016

College-aantekeningen bij Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology aan de Universiteit Leiden - 2015/2016


Hoorcollege 1: Wat zijn emoties en waarom hebben we ze?

Emoties in de volkspsychologie

Er is een groot verschil tussen wetenschappelijke kennis en ‘huis tuin en keuken’ psychologie. Een van de ideeën uit de volkspsychologie is dat je rationeel zou moeten zijn, en je niet moet laten leiden door je emoties. Een ander gebruikelijk idee in de volkspsychologie is dat ‘mijn hart me iets zegt, maar mijn verstand me iets anders vertelt’, en dat ‘emoties me gewoon overkomen’ of dat ‘tijd alle wonden heelt’.

Emoties: behulpzaam of schadelijk?

Emoties zijn heel belangrijke signalen, net als dat pijn ons vertelt dat er iets aan de hand is met je lichaam waar je aandacht aan moet besteden. Emoties vertellen je dat er iets betekenisvols is gebeurd. Al het andere verdwijnt dan even, emoties geven je een heel duidelijke focus. Dat is ook wat je communiceert naar je omgeving. Het ergste wat je kunt doen is je emoties negeren, net als dat het slecht is om pijn te negeren, omdat het dan alleen maar erger wordt.

Functionalistische emotietheorie

Frijda (1986) is een van de grondvaders van de huidige emotie theorie. Zijn theorie is tot op de dag van vandaag nog erg valide. Hij zegt dat er altijd iets gebeurt bij een emotie. Iets betekenisvols gebeurt, en dat kan ofwel een positieve ofwel een negatieve emotie zijn. Deze emoties, positief of negatief, veroorzaken veranderingen in actiebereidheid. Je kan vechten of vluchten, maar in beide situaties is het hele lichaam betrokken. Dit wordt fysieke opwinding genoemd, wat het lichaam voorbereidt om te reageren op de situatie. Het doel is om relaties te veranderen of te behouden. Dit is de kern van de hele emotie theorie: emoties zijn er omdat je iets wilt of juist iets niet wil in relatie tot anderen. Het gaat dus allemaal om de relaties met andere personen. We hebben emoties om onze relaties met anderen te reguleren.

Verdriet is een heel sterke emotie, en dit zorgt ervoor dat de focus van de persoon die ziet dat je verdrietig bent naar jou wordt verplaatst. Dus verdriet is eigenlijk bedoeld om aandacht te krijgen van iemand, zodat je geholpen wordt. Er bestaan tegenstrijdige emotietheorieën, maar het functionalistische perspectief is het meest belangrijke perspectief. Dit is het perspectief van Frijda.

De theorie van James-Lange (1884/85)

James-Lange heeft een ander standpunt wat betreft emoties. Volgens hem is er allereerst de perceptie van een.....read more

Access: 
Public
Summaries and study services for Psychology Bachelor 2/3 at Leiden University - Specialisation courses & Electives - Year 2022/2023

Summaries and study services for Psychology Bachelor 2/3 at Leiden University - Specialisation courses & Electives - Year 2022/2023

FSW building

This bundle contains relevent study materials with the second and third year of the Psychology Bachelor programme, in Dutch and English for Leiden University. Do you have your own lecture notes or summaries to share? Make your fellow students happy and upload them to your own WorldSupporter profile.

Work for WorldSupporter

Image

JoHo can really use your help!  Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world

Working for JoHo as a student in Leyden

Parttime werken voor JoHo

How to use more summaries?


Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

Using and finding summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

  1. Starting Pages: for some fields of study and some university curricula editors have created (start) magazines where customised selections of summaries are put together to smoothen navigation. When you have found a magazine of your likings, add that page to your favorites so you can easily go to that starting point directly from your profile during future visits. Below you will find some start magazines per field of study
  2. Use the menu above every page to go to one of the main starting pages
  3. Tags & Taxonomy: gives you insight in the amount of summaries that are tagged by authors on specific subjects. This type of navigation can help find summaries that you could have missed when just using the search tools. Tags are organised per field of study and per study institution. Note: not all content is tagged thoroughly, so when this approach doesn't give the results you were looking for, please check the search tool as back up
  4. Follow authors or (study) organizations: by following individual users, authors and your study organizations you are likely to discover more relevant study materials.
  5. Search tool : 'quick & dirty'- not very elegant but the fastest way to find a specific summary of a book or study assistance with a specific course or subject. The search tool is also available at the bottom of most pages

Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

Quicklinks to fields of study (main tags and taxonomy terms)

Field of study

Access level of this page
  • Public
  • WorldSupporters only
  • JoHo members
  • Private
Statistics
797 1
Comments, Compliments & Kudos:

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.