HC6+7: Bias
Medical research
Medical research often consists of the quantification of a phenomenon, for example quantifying the effect of cholesterol lowering treatment on the risk of cardiovascular events among those with elevated serum cholesterol levels. Ideally, results apply not only to those who participate in the study (the study population/sample), but also to others with elevated cholesterol levels (the domain).
The domain consists of a large group for whom the results of the study should apply. Of this domain, a sample is selected based on:
- In-/exclusion criteria
- Region
- Time period
- Et cetera
While the study is done within this sample, the results of the study should apply to the domain. If there is a systematic error, results of the study do not apply to the domain.
Systematic errors:
Systematic errors in design, conduct or analysis may lead to erroneous results → bias. An example of a systematic error is a broken measuring tape when measuring the length of something. Each time the measuring tape is used, results are incorrect → a systematic error is not solved by a large sample size.
Random errors:
Random errors can be decreased by increasing the sample size. As the study size increases, random errors start playing a increasingly smaller role.
Sources of bias
There are 3 sources of bias:
- Measurement errors → information bias
- Missing data → selection bias
- Incomparability of study groups → confounding
There can be bias in different parts of studies, depending on the study type:
- Randomized trial
- Outcome
- Cohort study
- Exposure
- Outcome
- Confounders
- Case-control study
- Exposure
- Outcome
- Confounders
Measurement errors
Examples of measurement errors are:
- Questionnaires for measuring body weight instead of scales
- Drug use measured by “prescription information” instead of controlling if the drugs actually are taken
- Measuring smoking status with “yes/no” instead of pack years
This changes the risk ratio → results of the study are (often) incorrect.
Consequences:
Measurement errors can have different consequences:
- Can result in over-/underestimation
- Better to prevent than to cure
- Correction is possible
Missing data
In case of missing data, certain information is not available:
- Questionnaire is not returned
- Question is not answered
- Study drop-outs
- Individuals not willing to participate
The extent of the impact of missing data often depends on the research question, study design, data collection, etc. This changes the risk ratio → a small percentage of missing data (3%) can lead to 100% bias, but this doesn’t necessarily happen → 50% missing data can also result in no bias (the ratio remains the same). Depending on if it is a selective or random process, missing data has a different influence on the results.
Selection bias:
Selection bias arises when in either of the exposure groups, a selection was made on stroke. Selecting on the outcome in a follow-up study hardly ever occurs since the participants have not yet developed the outcome at the start of follow-up.
Meta-analysis:
In meta-analysis, information is taken from multiple studies and is meta-analyzed → an average of all the available information is taken. For instance, some studies will be in favor of a certain treatment, while some are in favor of a placebo. These results are all taken into account and an average is made. Missing data in a meta-analysis may be caused by some studies not being reported → often happens if the results are in favor of the placebo. This leads to the meta-analysis showing that the drug is more effective, while it actually is not. This is called publication bias.
Consequences:
In conclusion, missing data:
- Can result in over-/underestimation
- Is better to prevent than cure
- Can be corrected → imputation
- This doesn’t solve the problem completely
Confounding
Confounding arises due to incomparability of exposure groups, for example if 1 exposure group is on average much older than the other exposure group. This may lead to the claim that an observed relation between exposure and outcome is causal, while it actually is not. The observed relation is partly or completely due to other reasons called “mixing of effects”, but does not represent a casual effect.
Incomparability:
If the mean age among exposed is 80 years, and the mean age among unexposed 60 years, the RR will be confounded → incomparability. The RR isn’t due to the exposure effect anymore, because the 2 groups are incomparable.
Conditions:
A confounder disrupts the relation between the exposure and the outcome. To be a confounding factor, 3 conditions must be met:
- Being associated with the exposure
- Being associated with the outcome
- Independently of exposure
- Not being an intermediate
- Cannot be a consequence of the exposure → not in the causal pathway
Numerical example:
The relation between the exposure and outcome of the following sample is investigated:
| Exposed | Unexposed |
Cases | 20 | 18 |
Non-cases | 20 | 22 |
| 40 | 40 |
Risk | 0,5 | 0,45 |
Risk ratio | 1,11 |
It needs to be investigated whether the RR of 1,11 represents the causal effect of exposure on the outcome and if the observed difference in outcomes between exposure groups is due to the exposure. The observed RR of 1,11 may be the result of incomparability between the exposure groups → confounding. To find out if this is the case, the sample is split in 2 groups of men and women individually.
Men | Exposed | Unexposed |
Cases | 2 | 9 |
Non-cases | 8 | 21 |
| 10 | 30 |
Risk | 0,2 | 0,3 |
Risk ratio | 0,67 |
Women | Exposed | Unexposed |
Cases | 18 | 9 |
Non-cases | 12 | 1 |
| 30 | 10 |
Risk | 0,6 | 0,9 |
Risk ratio | 0,67 |
These tables show the following:
- The risk of unexposed men developing the outcome is 30%, while the risk of unexposed women developing the outcome is 90% → gender is a risk factor for the outcome
- 25% of men are exposed and 75% of women are exposed → gender is not only a risk factor for the outcome, but is also related to exposure status
- The effect of exposure in both women and men is estimated to be around 0,67, while the overall estimated effect is 1,11
This shows that the overall RR is 1,11 and among men and women the RR is 0,67. The observed relation of 1,11 is a mixture of the “true” effect (0,67) and the effect of incomparability of exposure groups with respect to gender. Among men and women separately, exposure groups are comparable with respect to gender → in the subgroups, there is no confounding by gender anymore.
Does smoking increase the risk of a heart attack?
There is a relation between smoking and a heart attack:
- Heart attack: may be influenced by smoking and sex + age
- Smoking: influenced by sex + age, causes heart attacks
- Confounding factors: influences smoking and heart attacks
- Sex, age, stress, SES, etc.
- Mediators of the causal effects of smoking on heart attacks: blood pressure and cholesterol
- Not confounding variables
Several things can be done to prevent bias:
- Restriction: the study is only of elderly men
- By restricting the study population to a group of individuals who are all male and have the same age, confounding can be prevented
- A heart attack mainly occurs in elderly men and elderly men are more likely to smoke
- Downside: the sample is smaller and the results are less precise
- Stratification: analysis in subgroups
- All the possible subgroups are evaluated and a separate analysis within each subgroup is made
- Matching: creates comparability
- Statistical adjustment: regression analysis
- Makes a statistical adjustment for confounding
Matching:
In matching, for each individual who is exposed, a comparable individual who isn’t exposed is found → are taken as a pair and matched. This way, a data set is made with exposed and unexposed groups who are very comparable is made. Matching is done in both cohort studies and case-control studies:
- Cohort studies
- Exposed and non-exposed are matched with the aim to prevent confounding
- Analogy is done with randomized trials
- There only is comparability with respect to matched, observed, variables
- There is potential for unmeasured confounding
- Case-control studies
- Diseased and controls are made artificially → not always a good thing
- Diseased and controls should differ with respect to many (prognostic) factors
- Matching cases and controls in case-control studies is “unnatural” and overrated
- Can lead to is seeming like there no longer is an association between the exposure and the outcome → not the true effect
- Is used to increase the efficiency
- Can be matched or unmatched
- 100 cases of whom 95 are female
- 100 controls of whom 50 are female
- Unmatched: because more females have BRC than males, the gender ratio in cases will be different than the ratio in controls
- Within the subgroup of females → 95 cases : 50 controls
- Within the subgroup of males → 5 cases : 50 controls
- Matched
- Within the subgroup of females → 95 cases : 95 controls
- Within the subgroup of males → 5 cases : 5 controls
- Diseased and controls are made artificially → not always a good thing
Consequences:
Confounding can:
- Be relevant in observational studies of casual effects
- Can result in over-/underestimation
- Can be prevented
- E.g. with randomization
- Correction is possible, but only for measured confounding variables
- There is always potential for unmeasured confounding due to variables that were not measured
Risk difference
The risk difference is calculated by subtracting the cumulative incidence in the unexposed group (or least exposed group) from the cumulative incidence in the group with the exposure.
Number needed to harm:
The number needed to harm (NNH) is an epidemiological measure that indicates how many persons on average need to be exposed to a risk factor over a specific period to cause harm in an average of one person who would not otherwise have been harmed. It is calculated as follows:
- NNH = 1/risk difference
Join with a free account for more service, or become a member for full access to exclusives and extra support of WorldSupporter >>
Blok AWV2 2020/2021 UL
- Blok AWV HC1: Research questions
- Blok AWV HC2: RCT
- Blok AWV HC3: Sample size calculation
- Blok AWV HC4: Cohort studies
- Blok AWV HC5: Case control studies
- Blok AWV HC6+7: Bias
- Blok AWV HC8+9: Survival analysis
- Blok AWV HC10+11: Regression analysis
- Blok AWV HC12: Diagnostische begrippen
- Blok AWV HC13: Beslisbomen
- Blok AWV HC14: Test en behandeldrempel
Contributions: posts
Spotlight: topics
Blok AWV2 2020/2021 UL
Deze bundel bevat alle aantekeningen van de colleges uit het blok AWV uit het 2e jaar van de bachelor Geneeskunde aan de Universiteit Leiden. Ook aantekeningen uit de werkgroepen zijn in de samenvattingen verwerkt.
- Lees verder over Blok AWV2 2020/2021 UL
- 1664 keer gelezen
Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams
- Check out: Register with JoHo WorldSupporter: starting page (EN)
- Check out: Aanmelden bij JoHo WorldSupporter - startpagina (NL)
How and why use WorldSupporter.org for your summaries and study assistance?
- For free use of many of the summaries and study aids provided or collected by your fellow students.
- For free use of many of the lecture and study group notes, exam questions and practice questions.
- For use of all exclusive summaries and study assistance for those who are member with JoHo WorldSupporter with online access
- For compiling your own materials and contributions with relevant study help
- For sharing and finding relevant and interesting summaries, documents, notes, blogs, tips, videos, discussions, activities, recipes, side jobs and more.
Using and finding summaries, notes and practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter
There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.
- Use the summaries home pages for your study or field of study
- Use the check and search pages for summaries and study aids by field of study, subject or faculty
- Use and follow your (study) organization
- by using your own student organization as a starting point, and continuing to follow it, easily discover which study materials are relevant to you
- this option is only available through partner organizations
- Check or follow authors or other WorldSupporters
- Use the menu above each page to go to the main theme pages for summaries
- Theme pages can be found for international studies as well as Dutch studies
Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?
- Check out: Why and how to add a WorldSupporter contributions
- JoHo members: JoHo WorldSupporter members can share content directly and have access to all content: Join JoHo and become a JoHo member
- Non-members: When you are not a member you do not have full access, but if you want to share your own content with others you can fill out the contact form
Quicklinks to fields of study for summaries and study assistance
Main summaries home pages:
- Business organization and economics - Communication and marketing -International relations and international organizations - IT, logistics and technology - Law and administration - Leisure, sports and tourism - Medicine and healthcare - Pedagogy and educational science - Psychology and behavioral sciences - Society, culture and arts - Statistics and research
- Summaries: the best textbooks summarized per field of study
- Summaries: the best scientific articles summarized per field of study
- Summaries: the best definitions, descriptions and lists of terms per field of study
- Exams: home page for exams, exam tips and study tips
Main study fields:
Business organization and economics, Communication & Marketing, Education & Pedagogic Sciences, International Relations and Politics, IT and Technology, Law & Administration, Medicine & Health Care, Nature & Environmental Sciences, Psychology and behavioral sciences, Science and academic Research, Society & Culture, Tourisme & Sports
Main study fields NL:
- Studies: Bedrijfskunde en economie, communicatie en marketing, geneeskunde en gezondheidszorg, internationale studies en betrekkingen, IT, Logistiek en technologie, maatschappij, cultuur en sociale studies, pedagogiek en onderwijskunde, rechten en bestuurskunde, statistiek, onderzoeksmethoden en SPSS
- Studie instellingen: Maatschappij: ISW in Utrecht - Pedagogiek: Groningen, Leiden , Utrecht - Psychologie: Amsterdam, Leiden, Nijmegen, Twente, Utrecht - Recht: Arresten en jurisprudentie, Groningen, Leiden
JoHo can really use your help! Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world
1581 |
Add new contribution