Introduction to qualitative psychological research - an article by Coyle (2015)

Critical thinking
Article: Coyle, A (2015)
Introduction to qualitative psychological research

Introduction

This chapter examines the development of psychological interest in qualitative methods in historical context and point to the benefits that psychology gains from qualitative research.
It also looks at some important issues and developments in qualitative psychology.


Epistemology and the ‘scientific method’

At its most basic, qualitative psychological research may be regarded as involving the collection and analysis of non-numerical data through a psychological lens in order to provide rich descriptions and possibly explanations of peoples meaning-making, how they make sense of the world and how they experience particular events.

Qualitative research is bound up with particular sets of assumptions about the bases or possibilities of knowledge.
Epistemology: particular sets of assumptions about the bases or possibilities of knowledge.
Epistemology refers to a branch of philosophy that is concerned with the theory of knowledge and that tries to answer questions about how we can know what we know.
Ontology: the assumptions we make about the nature of being, existence or reality.

Different research approaches and methods are associated with different epistemologies.
The term ‘qualitative research’ covers a variety of methods with a range of epistemologies, resulting in a domain that is characterized by difference and tension.

The epistemology adopted by a particular study can be determined by a number of factors.

  • A researcher may have a favoured epistemological outlook or position and may locate their research within this, choosing methods that accord to with that position.
  • Alternatively, the researcher may be keen to use a particular qualitative method in their research and so they frame their study according to the epistemology that is usually associated with that method.

Whatever epistemological position is adopted in a study, it is usually desirable to ensure that you maintain this position consistently throughout the wire-up to help produce a coherent research report.

Positivism: holds that the relationship between the world and our sense perception of the world is straightforward. There is a direct correspondence between things in the world and our perception of them provided that our perception is not skewed by factors that might damage that correspondence.
So, it is possible to obtain accurate knowledge of things in the world, provided we can adopt an impartial, unbiased, objective viewpoint.

Empiricism: holds that our knowledge of the world must arise from the collection and categorization of our sense perceptions/observations of the world.
This categorization allows us to develop more complex knowledge of the world and to develop theories to explain the world.

  • Few scientists today adopt and unqualified positivist or empiricist outlook because it is generally recognized that our observations and perceptions do not provide pure and direct ‘facts’ about the world.

One fundamental claim from empiricism remains central in research, this is the idea that the development of knowledge requires the collection and analysis of data.
This is something shared by qualitative researchers, but they have different ideas about what constitutes appropriate data and about how those data should be generated and analysed.

Hypothetico-deductivism: the aim of research is not to obtain evidence that supports a theory but rather to identify theoretical claims (hyptheses) that are false and ultimately theories that are false.
Research that adopts a hypothetico-deductive stance therefore operates by developing hypotheses from theories and testing these hypotheses.

Deductive reasoning in research: reasoning begins with theories, which are refined into hypotheses, which are tested through observations of some sort, which leads to a confirmation or rejection of the hypotheses.
Top down.

Realism: the assumption that reality exists independent of the observer.

Resistance to the ‘scientific method’: alternative epistemologies and research foci

The ‘scientific method’ approach to psychological research has been resisted in some branches of the discipline.

Understanding individuals in context on their terms

Nomothetic research: approaches which seek generalizable findings that uncover laws to explain objective phenomena.

Idiographic research: approaches which seek to examine individual cases in detail to understand an outcome.

Phenomenological methods: focus on obtaining detailed descriptions of experience as understood by those who have that experience in order to discern its essence.
These methods are not concerned with producing an objective statement or an experience but rather with obtaining an individual’s personal perception or account of the experience on their own terms.

Inductive reasoning in research: reasoning that begins with data, which are examined in light of a study’s research questions.
Patterns in the data are discerned and labelled to theoretical levels.
Bottom up.

Any type of qualitative research that seeks to uncover people’s meanings and experiences in an inductive way has been described as embodying an ‘experimental’ approach.
Most of these forms of qualitative research have retained the realist commitment of the scientific method to some degree.

  • They assume that a reality exists independent of the observer which can be accessed in some way through research and that participants’ language provides us with a ‘window’ to that reality.

Critical realist outlook: assumes that, while a reality exits independent of the observer, we cannot know that reality with certainty.

Critical stance on the construction of reality

The social constructivist perspective adopts a critical stance towards the taken-for-granted ways in which we understand the world and ourselves.
For a social constructionist perspective, the ways in which we understand the world and ourselves are built up through social processes, especially through linguistic interactions, and so there is nothing fixed or necessary about them: they are the products of particular cultural and historical contexts.

Research constructed within a social constructionist framework focuses on examining the ways of constructing social reality that are available within a particular cultural and historical context, the conditions within which these ways of constructing are used as the implications they hold for human experience and practice.

Relativist: stance in which ‘reality’ is seen as dependent on the ways we come to know it.

Relativism and social constructionism contrast with the ontology and epistemology of other approaches to qualitative research which tend to assume that there is some relationship between the outcome of the analysis of research data and the actualities of which the analysis speaks.

Interpretative framework: ones professional and personal investments in the research.

Some qualitative methods that adopt a social constructionist epistemology hold on to the idea of data representing things that have an existence outside the data.
Others are largely disinterested in whether there is a reality existing ‘out there’ to which qualitative data correspond and instead locate their focus of interest elsewhere.

Summary

Realism

  • A reality exists independent of the observer and we can access this through research.
  • If a researcher has applied their research approach rigorously, they can be confident that their findings map on to the reality they were exploring.

Critical realism

  • A reality exists independent of the observer by we cannot know that reality with certainty.
  • A researcher should take care in moving beyond the realities of the participants and making claims about a reality that exists independently.

Relativism

  • ‘Reality’ is dependent on the ways we come to know it.
  • A researcher should ask how we come to build up versions of reality and should treat findings as versions of reality rather than as revealing realities independent of how we know them.

Reflexivity in qualitative research

Speaking position: including theoretical commitments, personal understandings and personal experiences.

Reflexivity: the acknowledgement by the researcher of the role played by their interpretative framework or speaking position in creating their analytic account.
The role of the researcher’s interpretative framework in generating data and producing the analysis is often regarded as a contaminating factor in most qualitative research, particularly the personal aspects of that framework.
In contrast, many qualitative methods are characterized by an expectation that the researcher will make explicit they speaking position.
When properly done, this can acknowledge the role of the researcher and it can increase the transparency of the research process and so help readers to understand and evaluate the work.

Evaluative criteria for qualitative research

A researcher’s acknowledgement of their speaking position within a study can help the reader to evaluate the research.
Here we evaluate how consumers of qualitative research can evaluate the worth of a qualitative study.

Positivist-empiricist, hypothetico-deductive, quantitative psychological research tends to be assessed in terms of criteria such as reliability and internal and external validity.
These rely on an assumption of objectivity, the researcher and research topic can be independent of each other.

Given the contention in most qualitative research that the researcher is inevitably present in their research, any evaluative criteria that relate to strategies for eliminating ‘bias’ are inappropriate.
In their research reports qualitative researchers may wish to specify alternative criteria by which they wish their research to be evaluated.
Criteria that should be held by good qualitative research:

  • Sensitivity to context
    Among other matters, the research should make clear the context of theory and the understandings created by previous researchers using similar methods and/or analysing similar topics, the socio-cultural setting of the study, and the social context of the relationship between the researchers and the participants.
  • Commitment
    Demonstrating prolonged engagement with the research topic
  • Rigour
    The completeness of the data collection and analysis
  • Transparency
    Detailing every aspect of the processes of data collection and analysis and disclosing/discussing all aspects of the research process
  • Coherence
    The quality of the research narrative, the ‘fit’ between the research question and the philosophical perspective adopted, and the method of investigation and analysis undertaken
  • Impact and importance
    The theoretical, practical and socio-cultural impact of the study.

Criteria applicable to qualitative and quantitative research:

  • Explicit scientific context and purpose
  • Appropriate methods
  • Respect for participants
  • Specification of methods
  • Appropriate discussion
  • Clarity of presentation
  • Contribution to knowledge

Combining research methods and approaches

It has become increasingly common to see both qualitative and quantitative methods being used in the same research object: mixed-methods approach.
This guards against methodolatry and can enrich research outcomes.
Quantitative research and qualitative research perform different functions and so a project that incorporates both can benefit from what each offers, most obviously breadth and depth.

What can be challenging is to integrate qualitative and quantitative findings that may have been generated by approaches and methods based on different epistemological assumptions.
But, if we are more modest about what integration involves, possibilities open up.

It should not be assumed that a combination of qualitative and quantitative methods is inherently superior to research that adopts a single approach.
The decision to use a combination of a quantitative and qualitative methods should be determined by how best to answer particular research question(s).

Pluralistic analysis: the value of applying different qualitative methods with different ontologies and epistemologies to a single data set.
The aim is to produce rich, multi-layered, multi-perspective readings of any qualitative data set through the application of diverse ‘ways of seeing’.

Check page access:
Public
Check more or recent content:

WSRt, critical thinking - a summary of all articles needed in the fourth block of second year psychology at the uva

Kinds versus continua: a review of psychometric approaches to uncover the structure of psychiatric constructs - summary of an article by Borsboom, Rhemtulla, Cramer, van der Maas, Scheffer and Dolan

Kinds versus continua: a review of psychometric approaches to uncover the structure of psychiatric constructs - summary of an article by Borsboom, Rhemtulla, Cramer, van der Maas, Scheffer and Dolan

Image

Critical thinking
Article: Borsboom, Rhemtulla, Cramer, van der Maas, Scheffer and Dolan (2016)
Kinds versus continua: a review of psychometric approaches to uncover the structure of psychiatric constructs

The present paper reviews psychometric modelling approaches that can be used to investigate the question whether psychopathology constructs are discrete or continuous dimensions through application of statistical models.


Introduction

The question of whether mental disorders should be thought of as discrete categories or as continua represents an important issue in clinical psychology and psychiatry.

  • The DSM-V typically adheres to a categorical model, in which discrete diagnoses are based on patterns of symptoms.

But, such categorizations often involve apparently arbitrary conventions.

Measurement theoretical definitions of kinds and continua

All measurement starts with categorization, the formation of equivalence classes.
Equivalence classes: sets of individuals who are exchangeable with respect to the attribute of interest.
We may not succeed in finding an observational procedure that in fact yields the desired equivalence classes.

  • We may find that individuals who have been assigned the same label are not indistinguishable with respect to the attribute of interest.
    Because there are now three classes rather than two, next to the relation between individuals within cases (equivalence), we may also represent systematic relations between members of different cases.
  • One may do so by invoking the concept of order.
    But, we may find that within these classes, there are non-trivial differences between individuals that we wish to represent.

If we break down the classes further, we may represent them with a scale that starts to approach continuity.

The continuity hypothesis formally implies that:

  • in between any two positions lies a third that can be empirically instantiated
  • there are no gaps in the continuum.

In psychological terms, categorical representations line up naturally with an interpretation of disorders as discrete disease entities, while continuum hypotheses are most naturally consistent with the idea that a construct varies continuously in a population.

  • in a continuous interpretation, the distinction between individuals depends on the imposition of a cut-off score that does not reflect a gap that is inherent in the attribute itself.

Kinds and continua as psychometric entities

In psychology, we have no way to decide conclusively whether two individuals are ‘equally depressed’.
This means we cannot form the equivalence classes necessary for measurement theory to operate.
The standard approach to dealing with this situation in psychology is

.....read more
Access: 
Public
Toward a Model-Based Approach to the Clinical Assessment of Personality Psychopathology - summary of an article by Eaton, Krueger, Docherty, and Sponheim

Toward a Model-Based Approach to the Clinical Assessment of Personality Psychopathology - summary of an article by Eaton, Krueger, Docherty, and Sponheim

Image

Critical thinking
Article: Eaton, Krueger, Docherty, and Sponheim (2013)
Toward a Model-Based Approach to the Clinical Assessment of Personality Psychopathology

This paper illustrates how new statistical methods can inform conceptualization of personality psychopathology and therefore its assessment.


The relationship between structure and assessment

Structural assumptions about personality variables are inextricably linked to personality assessment.

  • reliable assessment of normal-range personality traits, and personality disorder categories, frequently takes different forms, given that the constructs of interest are presumed to have different structures.
  • when assessing personality traits, the assessor needs to measure the full range of the trait dimension to determine where an individual falls in it.
  • then assessing the presence or absence of a DSM-V personality disorder, the assessor needs to evaluate the presence of absence of the binary categorical diagnosis.
  • given the polythetic nature of criterion sets, the purpose of the assessment is to determine which criteria are present, calculate the number of present criteria, and note whether this sum meets or exceeds a diagnostic threshold.

The nature of the personality assessment instrument reflect assumptions about the distributional characteristics of the construct of interest.

  • items on DSM-oriented inventories are usually intended to gather converging pieces of information about each criterion to determine whether or not it is present.

Distributional assumptions of personality constructs

Historically, many assumptions about the distributions of data reflecting personality constructs resulted form expert opinion or theory.
Both ‘type’ theories and dimensional theories have been proposed.
Assessment instruments have reflected this bifurcation in conceptualization.

  • The resulting implications for assessment are far from trivial
    The structure of a personality test designed to determine whether an individual is one or two personality types, needs only to assess the two characteristics, as opposed to assessing characteristics that are more indicative or mid-range.
    • There is no mid-ground in type theory, so items covering middle-ground are not relevant.

Because the structure of personality assessment is reflective of the underlying distributional assumptions of the personality constructs of interest, reliance solely on expert opinion about these distributions is potentially problematic.

Model-based tests of distributional assumptions

It is critical for personality theory and assessment that underlying distributional assumptions of symptomatology be correct and justifiable.

  • different distributions impact the way clinical and research constructs are conceptualized, measured, and applied to individuals.
  • characterizing these latent constructs properly is a prerequisite for efforts to asses them.
    • it is of limited
.....read more
Access: 
Public
Bayes and the probability of hypotheses - summary of Chapter 4 of Understanding Psychology as a science by Dienes

Bayes and the probability of hypotheses - summary of Chapter 4 of Understanding Psychology as a science by Dienes

Image

Critical thinking
Chapter 4 of Understanding Psychology as a science by Dienes
Bayes and the probability of hypotheses

Objective probability: a long-run relative frequency.
Classic (Neyman-Pearson) statistics can tell you the long-run relative frequency of different types of errors.

  • Classic statistics do not tell you the probability of any hypothesis being true.

An alternative approach to statistics is to start with what Bayesians say are people’s natural intuitions.
People want statistics to tell them the probability of their hypothesis being right.
Subjective probability: the subjective degree of conviction in a hypothesis.


Subjective probability

Subjective or personal probability: the degree of conviction we have in a hypothesis.
Probabilities are in the mind, not in the world.

The initial problem to address in making use of subjective probabilities is how to assign a precise number to how probable you think a proposition is.
The initial personal probability that you assign to any theory is up to you.
Sometimes it is useful to express your personal convictions in terms of odds rather than probabilities.

Odds(theory is true) = probability(theory is true)/probability(theory is false)
Probability = odds/(odds +1)

These numbers we get from deep inside us must obey the axioms of probability.
This is the stipulation that ensures the way we change our personal probability in a theory is coherent and rational.

  • People’s intuitions about how to change probabilities in the light of new information are notoriously bad.

This is where the statistician comes in and forces us to be disciplined.

There are only a few axioms, each more-or-less self-evidently reasonable.

  • Two aximons effectively set limits on what values probabilities can take.
    All probabilities will lie between 0 and 1
  • P(A or B) = P(A) + P(B), if A and B are mutually exclusive.
  • P(A and B) = P(A) x P(B|A)
    • P(B|A) is the probability of B given A.

Bayes’ theorem

H is the hypothesis
D is the data

P(H and D) = P(D) x P(H|D)
P(H and D) = P(H) x P(D|H)

so

P(D) x P(H|D) = P(H) x P(D|H)

Moving P(D) to the other side

P(H|D) = P(D|H) x P(H) / P(D)

This last one is Bayes theorem.
It tells you how to go from one conditional probability to its inverse.
We can simplify this equation if we are interested in comparing the probability of different hypotheses given the same data D.
Then P(D) is just a constant for all these comparisons.

P(H|D) is proportional to P(D|H) x

.....read more
Access: 
Public
Bayesian Versus orthodox statistics: which side are you on? - summary of an article by Dienes, 2011

Bayesian Versus orthodox statistics: which side are you on? - summary of an article by Dienes, 2011

Image

Critical thinking
Article: Dienes, Z, 2011
Bayesian Versus orthodox statistics: which side are you on?
doi: 10.1177/1745691611406920


The contrast: orthodox versus Bayesian statistics

The orthodox logic of statistics, starts from the assumption that probabilities are long-run relative frequencies.
A long-run relative frequency requires an indefinitely large series of events that constitutes the collective probability of some property (q) occurring is then the proportion of events in the collective with property q.

  • The probability applies to the whole collective, not to any one person.
    • One person may belong to two different collectives that have different probabilities
  • Long run relative frequencies do not apply to the truth of individual theories because theories are not collectives. They are just true or false.
    • Thus, when using this approach to probability, the null hypothesis of no population difference between two particular conditions cannot be assigned a probability.
  • Given both a theory and a decision procedure, one can determine a long-run relative frequency with which certain data might be obtained. We can symbolize this as P(data| theory and decision procedure).

The logic of Neyman Pearson (orthodox) statistics is to adopt decision procedures with known long-term error rates and then control those errors at acceptable levels.

  • Alpha: the error rate for false positives, the significance level
  • Beta: the error rate for false negatives

Thus, setting significance and power controls long-run error rates.

  • An error rate can be calculated from the tail area of test statistics.
  • An error rate can be adjusted for factors that affect long-run error rates
  • These error rates apply to decision procedures, not to individual experiments.
    • An individual experiment is a one-time event, so does not constitute a long-run set of events
    • A decision procedure can in principle be considered to apply over a indefinite long-run number of experiments.

The probabilities of data given theory and theory given data

The probability of a theory being true given data can be symbolized as P(theory|data).
This is what orthodox statistics tell us.
One cannot infer one conditional probability just by knowing its inverse. (So P(data|theory) is unknown).

Bayesian statistics starts from the premise that we

.....read more
Access: 
Public
Network Analysis: An Integrative Approach to the Structure of Psychopathology - summary of an article by Borsboom and Cramer (2013)

Network Analysis: An Integrative Approach to the Structure of Psychopathology - summary of an article by Borsboom and Cramer (2013)

Image

Critical thinking
Article: Borsboom, D. and Cramer, A, O, J. (2013)
Network Analysis: An Integrative Approach to the Structure of Psychopathology
doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185608


Introduction

The current dominant paradigm of the disease model of psychopathology is problematic.
Current handling of psychopathology data is predicated on traditional psychometric approaches that are the technical mirror of of this paradigm.
In these approaches, observables (clinical symptoms) are explained by means of a small set of latent variables, just like symptoms are explained by disorders.

  • From this psychometric perspective, symptoms are regarded as measurements of a disorder, and in accordance, symptoms are aggregated in a total score that reflects a person’s stance on that latent variable.
  • The dominant paradigm is not merely a matter of theoretical choice, but also of methodological and pragmatic necessity.

In this review, we argue that complex network approaches, which are currently being developed at the crossroads of various scientific fields, have the potential to provide a way of thinking about disorders that does justice to their complex organisation.

  • In such approaches, disorders are conceptualized as systems of causally connected symptoms rather than as effects of a latent disorder.
  • Using network analysis techniques, such systems can be represented, analysed, and studied in their full complexity.
  • In addition, network modeling has the philosophical advantage of dropping the unrealistic idea that symptoms of a single disorder share a single causal background, while it simultaneously avoids the realistic consequence that disorders are merely labels for an arbitrary set of symptoms.
    • It provides a middle ground in which disorders exists as systems, rather than as entities

Symptoms and disorders in psychopathology

We know for certain that people suffer from symptoms and that these symptoms cluster in a non-arbitrary way.
For most psychopathological conditions, the symptoms are only empirically identifiable causes of distress.

  • Mental disorders are themselves not empirically identifiable in that they cannot be diagnosed independently of their symptoms.
    • It is impossible to identify any of the common mental disorders as conditions that exists independently of their symptoms.

In order for a disease model to hold, it should be possible to conceptually separate conditions from symptoms.

  • It must be possible (or at least imaginable) that a person should have a condition/disease without the associated symptoms.

This isn’t possible for mental disorders.
As an important corollary, this means that disorders cannot be causes of these

.....read more
Access: 
Public
Introduction to qualitative psychological research - an article by Coyle (2015)

Introduction to qualitative psychological research - an article by Coyle (2015)

Image

Critical thinking
Article: Coyle, A (2015)
Introduction to qualitative psychological research

Introduction

This chapter examines the development of psychological interest in qualitative methods in historical context and point to the benefits that psychology gains from qualitative research.
It also looks at some important issues and developments in qualitative psychology.


Epistemology and the ‘scientific method’

At its most basic, qualitative psychological research may be regarded as involving the collection and analysis of non-numerical data through a psychological lens in order to provide rich descriptions and possibly explanations of peoples meaning-making, how they make sense of the world and how they experience particular events.

Qualitative research is bound up with particular sets of assumptions about the bases or possibilities of knowledge.
Epistemology: particular sets of assumptions about the bases or possibilities of knowledge.
Epistemology refers to a branch of philosophy that is concerned with the theory of knowledge and that tries to answer questions about how we can know what we know.
Ontology: the assumptions we make about the nature of being, existence or reality.

Different research approaches and methods are associated with different epistemologies.
The term ‘qualitative research’ covers a variety of methods with a range of epistemologies, resulting in a domain that is characterized by difference and tension.

The epistemology adopted by a particular study can be determined by a number of factors.

  • A researcher may have a favoured epistemological outlook or position and may locate their research within this, choosing methods that accord to with that position.
  • Alternatively, the researcher may be keen to use a particular qualitative method in their research and so they frame their study according to the epistemology that is usually associated with that method.

Whatever epistemological position is adopted in a study, it is usually desirable to ensure that you maintain this position consistently throughout the wire-up to help produce a coherent research report.

Positivism: holds that the relationship between the world and our sense perception of the world is straightforward. There is a direct correspondence between things in the world and our perception of them provided that our perception is not skewed by factors that might damage that correspondence.
So, it is possible to obtain accurate knowledge of things in the world, provided we can adopt an impartial, unbiased, objective viewpoint.

Empiricism: holds that our knowledge of the world must arise from the collection and categorization of our sense perceptions/observations of the world.
This categorization allows us to develop more complex knowledge

.....read more
Access: 
Public
Surrogate Science: The Idol of a Universal Method for Scientific Inference - summary of an article by Gigerenzer & Marewski

Surrogate Science: The Idol of a Universal Method for Scientific Inference - summary of an article by Gigerenzer & Marewski

Image

Critical thinking
Article: Gigerenzer, G. & Marewski, J, N. (2015)
Surrogate Science: The Idol of a Universal Method for Scientific Inference
doi: 10.1177/0149206314547522

Introduction

Scientific inference should not be made mechanically.
Good science requires both statistical tools and informed judgment about what model to construct, what hypotheses to test, and what tools to use.

This article is about the idol of a universal method of statistical inference.

In this article, we make three points:

  • There is no universal method of scientific inference, but, rather a toolbox of useful statistical methods. In the absence of a universal method, its followers worship surrogate idols, such as significant p values.
    The inevitable gap between the ideal and its surrogate is bridged with delusions.
    These mistaken beliefs do much harm. Among others, by promoting irreproducible results.
  • If the proclaimed ‘Bayesian revolution’ were to take place, the danger is that the idol of a universal method might survive in a new guise, proclaiming that all uncertainty can be reduced to subjective probabilities.
  • Statistical methods are not simply applied to a discipline. They change the discipline itself, and vice versa.

Dreaming up a universal method of inference

The null ritual

The most prominent creation of a seemingly universal inference method is the null ritual:

  • Set up a null hypothesis of ‘no mean inference’ or ‘zero correlation’. Do not specify the predictions or your own research hypothesis.
  • Use 5% as a convention for rejecting the null. If significant, accept you research hypothesis. Report the result as p<.05, p<.01, p<.001, whichever comes next to the obtained p value.
  • Always perform this procedure.

Level of significance has three different meanings:

  • A mere convention
  • The alpha level
  • The exact level of significance

Three meanings of significance

The alpha level: the long-term relative frequency of mistakenly rejecting hypothesis H0 if it is true, also known as Type I error rate.
The beta level: the long-term frequency of mistakenly rejecting H1 if it is true.

Two statistical hypothesis need to be specified in order to be able to determine both alpha and beta.
Neyman and Pearson rejected a mere convention in favour of an alpha level that required a rational scheme.

  • Set up two statistical hypotheses, H1, H2, and decide on alpha, beta and the sample size before the experiment, based on subjective cost-benefit considerations.
  • If the data fall
.....read more
Access: 
Public
WSRt, critical thinking, a list of terms used in the articles of block 4

WSRt, critical thinking, a list of terms used in the articles of block 4

Image

This is a list of the important terms used in the articles of the fourth block of WSRt, with the subject alternative approaches to psychological research.


Article: Kinds versus continua: a review of psychometric approaches to uncover the structure of psychiatric constructs

Equivalence classes: sets of individuals who are exchangeable with respect to the attribute of interest.

Taxometrics: by inspecting particular consequences of the model for specific statistical properties of (subsets of) items, such as the patterns of bivariate correlations expected to hold in the data

Toward a Model-Based Approach to the Clinical Assessment of Personality Psychopathology

Latent trait models: posit the presence of one or more underlying continuous distributions.

Zones of rarity: locations along the dimension that are unoccupied by some individuals.

Discrimination: the measure of how strongly the item taps into the latent trait.

Quasi-continuous: the construct would be bounded at the low end by zero, a complete absence of the quality corresponding with the construct.

Latent class models: based on the supposition of a latent group (class) structure for a construct’s distribution.

Conditional independence: that inter-item correlations solely reflect class membership.

Hybrid models (of factor mixture models): combine the continuous aspects of latent trait models with the discrete aspects of latent class models.

EFMA: exploratory factor mixture analysis.

Bayes and the probability of hypotheses

Objective probability: a long-run relative frequency.

Subjective probability: the subjective degree of conviction in a hypothesis.

The likelihood principle: the notion that all the information relevant to inference contained in data is provided by the likelihood.

Probability density distribution: the distribution of if the dependent variable can be assumed to vary continuously

Credibility interval: the Bayesian equivalent of a confidence interval

The Bayes factor: the Bayesian equivalent of null hypothesis testing

Flat prior or uniform prior: you have no idea what the population value is likely to be

Bayesian

.....read more
Access: 
JoHo members
Everything you need for the course WSRt of the second year of Psychology at the Uva

Everything you need for the course WSRt of the second year of Psychology at the Uva

Image

This magazine contains all the summaries you need for the course WSRt at the second year of psychology at the Uva.

Access: 
Public
What is a confidence interval in null hypothesis significance testing?
What are important elements of Bayesian statistics?
What is the Bayes factor?

What is the Bayes factor?

Image

The Bayes factor (B) compares the probability of an experimental theory to the probability of the null hypothesis.
It gives the means of adjusting your odds in a continuous way.

  • If B is greater than 1, your data support the experimental hypothesis over the null
  • If B is less than 1, your data support the null over the experimental hypothesis
  • If B is about 1, then your experiment was not sensitive

For more information, look at the (free) summary of 'Bayes and the probability of hypotheses' or 'Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: which side are you one?'

What are weaknesses of the Bayesian approach?

What are weaknesses of the Bayesian approach?

Image

Weaknesses of the Bayesian approach are:

  • The prior is subjective
  • Bayesian analysis force people to consider what a theory actually predicts, but specifying the predictions in detail may by contentious
  • Bayesian analysis escape the paradoxes of violating the likelihood principle, but in doing so they no longer control for Type I and Type II errors

For more information, look at the (free) summary of 'Bayesian versus orthodox statistics: which side are you on?'

 

What is qualitative psychological research?

What is qualitative psychological research?

Image

At its most basic, qualitative psychological research can be seen as involving the collection and analysis of non-numerical data through a psychological lens in order to provide rich descriptions and possibly explanations of peoples meaning-making, how they make sense of the world and how they experience particular events.

For more information, look at the (free) summary of 'Introduction to qualitative psychological research'

What criteria should be held by good qualitative research?
Work for WorldSupporter

Image

JoHo can really use your help!  Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world

Working for JoHo as a student in Leyden

Parttime werken voor JoHo

Check more of this topic?
How to use more summaries?


Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

Using and finding summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

  1. Starting Pages: for some fields of study and some university curricula editors have created (start) magazines where customised selections of summaries are put together to smoothen navigation. When you have found a magazine of your likings, add that page to your favorites so you can easily go to that starting point directly from your profile during future visits. Below you will find some start magazines per field of study
  2. Use the menu above every page to go to one of the main starting pages
  3. Tags & Taxonomy: gives you insight in the amount of summaries that are tagged by authors on specific subjects. This type of navigation can help find summaries that you could have missed when just using the search tools. Tags are organised per field of study and per study institution. Note: not all content is tagged thoroughly, so when this approach doesn't give the results you were looking for, please check the search tool as back up
  4. Follow authors or (study) organizations: by following individual users, authors and your study organizations you are likely to discover more relevant study materials.
  5. Search tool : 'quick & dirty'- not very elegant but the fastest way to find a specific summary of a book or study assistance with a specific course or subject. The search tool is also available at the bottom of most pages

Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

Quicklinks to fields of study (main tags and taxonomy terms)

Field of study

Access level of this page
  • Public
  • WorldSupporters only
  • JoHo members
  • Private
Statistics
1988
Comments, Compliments & Kudos:

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.
Promotions
vacatures

JoHo kan jouw hulp goed gebruiken! Check hier de diverse studentenbanen die aansluiten bij je studie, je competenties verbeteren, je cv versterken en een bijdrage leveren aan een tolerantere wereld

More contributions of WorldSupporter author: SanneA:
Follow the author: SanneA