An Introduction to Developmental psychology by A. Slater and G. Bremner (third edition) - a summary
- 3675 reads
Join with a free account for more service, or become a member for full access to exclusives and extra support of WorldSupporter >>
Developmental psychology
Chapter 6
Emotional development and attachment relationships
Introduction
Emotional development underlies many other aspects of development, and has serious implications for how we conduct research with children.
Children’s emotional development can broadly be divided into three areas.
Expressing and recognizing emotional expressions
Are expressions of emotions innate?
Cross-cultural evidence
There is good evidence for the universality of human facial expressions of emotion.
Understanding of how emotions are conveyed through facial expressions is universal, but does not necessarily mean that understanding emotional expressions is innate.
Expressions of emotion in infancy
Infants from birth spontaneously display a wide repertoire of emotions though their facial expressions.
Basic emotions: happiness, interest, surprise, disgust, sadness, distress, anger, fear.
Complex emotions: pride, shyness, jealousy, guilt, shame, embarrassment.
Adults are skillful in accurately reading infants’ expressions.
However, adults are less accurate in discriminating infant’s negative facial expressions indicative of fear, anger, sadness or disgust. This appears not to be due to a lack of subtlety in young infants’ expression, but to the fact that the facial expressions arising from these different emotions are quite similar.
There is a biological basis for infant’s emotional facial expressions.
Multiple facial cues are used to signal emotion and the ability to convey and accurately interpret emotional expressions is impressively robust.
Infants indisputably display basic emotions very early in life. But there is considerable debate about when complex emotions emerge.
Infant discrimination of facial expressions
3-month-olds can distinguish between photographs of people smiling and frowning.
4- to 7-month-olds can distinguish between expressions of happiness and surprise.
Can young infants empathize with others’ emotions?
Very young infants may be emphasizing with the emotion they see portrayed.
But we cannot be sure.
Social referencing
Social referencing: infants and young children look at their caregiver for ‘advice’ when faced with an difficult or uncertain situation and seek social cures to guide their actions.
This provides and excellent way to assess infants’ understanding of other people’s emotional expressions.
Emotion understanding
Children begin to talk about emotions at a surprisingly young age, and parents readily give anecdotal accounts of their children using emotion words in the second year of life.
There are differences between emotional responses of infants and young children and those of older children and adults.
Emotional ambiguity: the realization that a person’s feelings may not be clear-cut or match your own emotional response.
Emotion understanding tasks
There are links between young children’s task-based understanding of emotions and their naturalistic behavior during play.
Emotional understanding may be related to children’s ability to form harmonious close relationships with others.
There is evidence that infants as young as 18 months can appreciate that another person might like something that they themselves dislike.
4 year olds could not use the capacity of understanding false belief to predict the likely emotional response, and thus appeared unable to integrate their understanding of beliefs and emotions.
By age 6 they can.
Up to age 5 children are typically able to understand the public aspects of emotion.
Around age 7, children understand the mentalistic nature of emotions.
Between 9 and 11 years of age, children realize that one can feel two emotions in response to the same event, that cognitive strategies can be used to regulate emotions and that morals and moral transgressions relate to emotional responses.
There is a cross-cultural stability in the development of emotion understanding.
Factors relating to emotion understanding
There is a positive correlation between children’s emotion understanding and concurrent performance on theory of mind tasks.
There is evidence that emotion understanding is acquired earlier than theory of mind understanding. And emotion understanding may facilitate children’s acquisition of theory of mind abilities.
The quality of family interaction also relates to children’s emotion understanding.
A tendency to discuss people’s feelings and the causes of such emotions is related to children’s understanding of the emotional states of story characters and emotional conflicts.
Children are more likely to understand the relation between false belief and emotion if their mothers focuses on their mental characteristics when describing them.
Caregivers’ behavior early in the childhood has been found to predict children’s later emotion understanding.
Mind-mindedness: caregivers who are able to ‘read’ their infant’s signals appropriately. Maternal mind-mindedness is a good predictor of attachment security.
Callous-unemotional traits.
Include:
High levels of CU traits in middle school and adolescence are associated with problems in emotion processing and emphasizing with others. These problems are specific to emotion understanding. (theory of mind is not a problem).
Interventions to improve these children’s emotion understanding are effective.
Emotion regulation
Emotion regulation: adjusting one’s emotional state to a suitable level of intensity. This prevents emotional ‘overload’ and allows one to function in a consistent manner.
Children as young as 3 years old show some ability to control their expression of mild negative emotions in a test situation.
There is evidence that, as early as the second year of life, children have begun to regulate their displays of emotion.
Reasons:
Children’s tendency to feel guilty after staged missteps positively correlates with their ability to regulate their behavior.
Children’s conscience development between 2 and 5 years of age predicted their later competence and social behavior.
Factors that predict children’s conscience development:
Shared cooperation and positive affect between parent and child and parental responsiveness to the child.
Bowlby’s theory of attachment
Attachment as a innate drive
The infant’s expression of emotion and the caregiver’s response of these emotions lies at the heart of John Bowlby’s theory of attachment.
Before Bowlby, the predominant view of infant-mother attachment was that it was a secondary drive (a by product of the infant associating the mother with providing for physiological needs).
Bowlby argued that attachment was an innate primary drive in the infant.
Bowlby recognized that the establishment of an attachment relationship was not dependent purely upon the social and environmental interplay between infant and caregiver.
It depends on the infant’s level of cognitive development in terms of being able to represent an object that is not physically present.
The phases of attachment
attachments develop in phases:
Two most widely used instruments in developmental psychology to investigate Bowlby’s theoretical claims.
Mary Ainsworth and the strange situation procedure
Maternal deprivation: a term to describe the deprivation infants experience as result of long-term separation from their mother, or from being orphaned.
Strange situation procedure.
Typically conducted when the infant is between 1 and 2 years of age.
In the testing room, there are two chairs, one for the caregiver and one for the stranger, and a range of toys with which the infant can play.
Procedure:
Infant’s responses during the two reunion episodes are most crucial, and form the basis for assessing and infant’s security of attachment.
Infant behavior is described according to four indices:
Different attachment types
Secure attachments
Insecure avoidant
Insecure-resistant
Insecure disorganized
The majority of infants are classified as secure, but over a third of infants fall into the three insecure attachment categories, so insecure attachment should not be regarded as abnormal.
Factors predicting attachment security
Individual differences in the caregiver’s sensitivity to the infant’s cues were the earliest reported predictors of attachment security.
Mothers who responded most sensitively to their infants’ cues during the first year of life tended subsequently to have securely attached infants.
The insecure-avoidant pattern of attachment was associated with mothers who tended to reject or ignore the infants’ cues. And inconsistent patterns of mothering were related to insecure-resistant attachment
Mothers of securely attached infants appeared to be capable of perceiving things form the child’s point of view.
Mind-mindedness has been shown to be a better predictor of attachment security.
Mind-mindedness can distinguish between the four attachment groups.
Internal working models and the adult attachment interview
Attachment theory proposes that children use their early experiences with their caregivers to form internal working models which incorporate representations of themselves, their caregivers and their relationships with others. The child will then use these these internal working models as templates for interacting with others.
Attachment categories in adults
Individuals’ representations of their early childhood experiences with attachment figures are assessed during the Adult Attachment Interview, a semi-structured interview in which adults are asked to describe their childhood relationships with mother and father, and to recall times when they were separated from their parents or felt upset or rejected.
Adults are placed into one of four attachment categories. There are based on the way their represent their experiences, be they good or bad.
Autonomous
Dismissing
Preoccupied
Unresolved
Assessing internal models in children
Researches have developed various methods to assess internal working models of attachment during childhood. These methods are known as representational measures of attachment.
Can be used with children from age 3 upwards.
They assess attachment security in terms of children’s reaction to emotionally provocative themes that aim to trigger an attachment-related response. These themes can be presented in picture or photograph format, or as the beginning of stories. The child is asked how the story will end or what the child in the picture will do.
Attachment security is assigned on the basis of whether the child recognizes the distress, and the extend to which the child uses attachment figures to assuage any distress.
There is no definitive way of establishing whether the child’s responses in these tasks are actually anchored in their own experiences of attachment relationships.
Parental AAI classification and infant-parent attachment
Parental AAI classifications have been found to relate systematically to the security of the infant-parent attachment relationship.
The way in which a parent represents their own childhood attachment experiences is related to the types of relationship formed with their own children.
Longitudinal stability in attachment security
Attachment security remains stable in only about half of the infants over a 6-month period (12 to 18 months).
There is little evidence to suggest that patterns of attachment remain stable over either relatively short or very long periods of time.
This bundle contains a summary of the book An Introduction to Developmental psychology by A. Slater and G. Bremner (third edition). The book is about development from fetus to elderly. Only the chapters needed in the course 'Developmental psychology' in the first year of
...There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.
Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?
Field of study
JoHo can really use your help! Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world
3190 |
Add new contribution