“Nosek, Spies, & Motyl (2012). Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability.” - Article summary

There is a publication bias in research. Mostly positive results – that is significant – are being published and non-significant results are often not published. This leads to researchers only trying to find significant results instead of genuine effects or non-effects.

Researchers are more likely to find significant results because of flexible analysis options (1), the confirmation bias (2) and the drive to find significant results (3). It is possible to find significant results everywhere if you search hard enough. Direct replications do not occur often in research and are almost never published.

There are several practices that can increase publishability but can decrease the validity of the results:

  1. Leveraging chance by running many low-powered studies instead of a few high-powered ones.
  2. Uncritically dismissing “failed” studies as pilot studies but uncritically accepting “successful” studies.
  3. Selectively reporting studies with positive results and not studies with negative results (cherry picking).
  4. Stopping data collection as soon as a reliable effect is obtained.
  5. Continuing data collection until a reliable effect is obtained.
  6. Including multiple independent and/or dependent variable and reporting the subset that “worked”.
  7. Maintaining flexibility in design and analytical models (attempt to exclude data)
  8. Reporting a discovery as if it had been the result of a confirmatory test
  9. Not doing a direct replication once a reliable effect is obtained

Conceptual replication involves deliberately changing the operationalization of the key elements of the design such as the independent variable, dependent variable or both. Demonstrating the same effects with multiple operationalizations provides confidence in its conceptual interpretation. Conceptual replication is not an effective replacement for direct replication.

The peer review process offers a way to detect false results. Not publishing articles without replications would also be effective in lessening the publication bias and the number of false results in science. This could also be ineffective, because it could reduce the number of innovative research as scientists might ‘play it on safe’.

Paradigm-driven research can be used to both confirm and disconfirm prior results. It accumulates knowledge by systematically altering a procedure to investigate a theory or a research question. This includes both replication and extension of the research. One pitfall of paradigm-driven research is that the research could become about the methodology, instead of the theory. Conceptual replication prevents this.

Check-lists could provide effective when conducting research as this prevents information from being left out. A metric that determines what is worth replicating could also prove to be effective. Resource constraints to replicate some findings could be overcome by using crowdfunding. Peer reviewers judge whether a finding is important enough to be published, but they are not always capable of making that judgement.

Another solution to the publication bias and the number of false results in science is to shift the attention from publishing – make publishing trivial – to the evaluation of the research.

The solution to the previously described problems would be open data (1), open materials (2) and open workflow (3).

Open data refers to sharing the data which allows other researchers to confirm the data. Making data open increases the likelihood of finding and correcting errors and ultimately improving reported results. Data openness can be a problem when the data cannot ensure anonymity of the participant’s identity or when the data is going to be used for a lot of research projects and the data collection process is intense.

Open methods and materials can have the same effect as open data and can also facilitate progress in reuse, adaptation and extension for new research. The availability of the materials and methods will also speed up productivity by eliminating the need to reinvent or recreate them.

Open workflow refers to openness of the research process. This could include preregistering the research process, what statistical analyses will be used and so on. This makes it more difficult to hide undesirable results.

Image

Access: 
Public

Image

Join WorldSupporter!
This content is used in:

Scientific & Statistical Reasoning – Summary interim exam 2 (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Scientific & Statistical Reasoning – Article summary (UNIVERSITY OF AMSTERDAM)

Search a summary

Image

 

 

Contributions: posts

Help other WorldSupporters with additions, improvements and tips

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.

Image

Spotlight: topics

Check the related and most recent topics and summaries:
Institutions, jobs and organizations:
Activities abroad, study fields and working areas:
This content is also used in .....

Image

Check how to use summaries on WorldSupporter.org

Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

How and why use WorldSupporter.org for your summaries and study assistance?

  • For free use of many of the summaries and study aids provided or collected by your fellow students.
  • For free use of many of the lecture and study group notes, exam questions and practice questions.
  • For use of all exclusive summaries and study assistance for those who are member with JoHo WorldSupporter with online access
  • For compiling your own materials and contributions with relevant study help
  • For sharing and finding relevant and interesting summaries, documents, notes, blogs, tips, videos, discussions, activities, recipes, side jobs and more.

Using and finding summaries, notes and practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

  1. Use the summaries home pages for your study or field of study
  2. Use the check and search pages for summaries and study aids by field of study, subject or faculty
  3. Use and follow your (study) organization
    • by using your own student organization as a starting point, and continuing to follow it, easily discover which study materials are relevant to you
    • this option is only available through partner organizations
  4. Check or follow authors or other WorldSupporters
  5. Use the menu above each page to go to the main theme pages for summaries
    • Theme pages can be found for international studies as well as Dutch studies

Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

Quicklinks to fields of study for summaries and study assistance

Main summaries home pages:

Main study fields:

Main study fields NL:

Follow the author: JesperN
Work for WorldSupporter

Image

JoHo can really use your help!  Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world

Working for JoHo as a student in Leyden

Parttime werken voor JoHo

Statistics
2199