Join with a free account for more service, or become a member for full access to exclusives and extra support of WorldSupporter >>

Image

How do you keep mental account of gains, losses and regret? – Chapter 32

For most people, gaining money reflects achievement and self-regard. We keep score in our mind when we lose or gain money and consider them punishments and rewards, threats and promises. The ‘scores’ motivate our actions and influence our preferences. Cutting our losses feels like a failure, so we refuse doing it.

We hold money in both physical and mental accounts. We have general savings, special savings for our kids or emergencies and money to spend. We are more willing to draw on some of these accounts compared to other accounts to cover current needs. Self-control also plays a role: some accounts can be used only for household expenses, they limit have much we can spend. Mental accounts are a form of narrow framing: they keep things manageable and under control. Mental accounts are used for keeping score. For instance, successful golfers have a separate account for each hole, not just one for their overall score.

Consider the following example of mental accounting. Two Rihanna fans have to travel 50 miles to visit her concert. One of them bought the ticket, the other one got it as a gift. A heavy snowstorm is announced for the day of the concert. Which of the two fans is more likely to brave the storm to see Rihanna? The fan who paid for the ticket. Both of them set up a mental account for the concert they hoped to visit. Missing the concert will result into a negative balance. Both will be sad, but the closing balance is more negative for the fan who paid for the ticket. System 1 performs the calculations of emotional balance. For System 2 to respond rationally, it would have to be aware of the counterfactual possibility: “Would I still travel in this blizzard if I had gotten the ticket as a gift?” This requires a disciplined and active mind.

Imagine an individual investor having to sell stock because he is in need of money. He still remembers the price at which he bought each stock and can identify it as a loser or a winner. Fafsung is a winner: if he sells it now, he gains € 8.000. He holds an equal investment in Fapple, which is now worth € 8.000 less than he paid for it. In the last month, the value of both stocks has been stable. Which is he more likely to sell? Closing the Fafsung account results into an positive score. Closing the Fapple account would lead to a negative score. He has to choose between pleasure and pain, so he will likely sell Fafsung. Financial research shows that there is a major preference for selling winners. This bias is called the ‘disposition effect’, which is an example of narrow framing. The state of the mental account was considered a valid consideration for selling. If you care more about your wealth, you would sell the loser.

Imagine a firm that has already spent € 20 million on a failing project. The forecasts are worse than at the starting point. Another € 40 million is needed to give it a chance. The alternative is investing the money in a new project that appears to have a brighter future. What will the firm do? Companies often refuse to accept the humiliation of closing the account of the failure and invest more money in the failing project. In light of the fourfold pattern: this represents the choice between an unfavorable gamble and a sure loss. The sunk-cost fallacy keeps people too long in unhappy relationships, bad jobs and unpromising projects.

When do we feel the most regret?

Regret is something we consider a punishment. The fear of regret is a factor in a lot of decisions we make. Regret is triggered by the availability of alternatives to reality. Regret differs from blame, but both are induced by a comparison to a norm.

We tend to feel greater regret we experience after acting than after failing to act. Consider this example: “Anna considered switching employers, but decided not to switch. She learns that she would have been better off € 2.500 if she had switched. Claire has switched employers and learns she would have been better off by € 2.500 if she had kept her job by her former employer. Who feels the most regret?” Most people would say Claire, because she acted. People expect to have stronger emotional responses, like regret, to an outcome produced by action than by inaction. This is also found in the context of gambling.

When it comes to the endowment effect, reactions to price changes and choices between gambles, losses are weighted approximately twice as much as gains. In certain situations, the loss-aversion coefficient is a lot higher. An example is health. Consider the following problem. “You have been exposed to a disease, which if contracted leads to a rapid and painless death within 4 days. The probability that you contracted it is 1/1.200. Before symptoms occur, you can get an effective vaccine. What is the maximum you would be willing to pay for it?” The majority of people would be willing to buy a limited amount. The risk of dying is small, it appears unreasonable to pay a lot of money in order to avoid it. Now consider the following problem: “For research on diseases, you are asked to expose yourself to a 1/1.200 chance of contracting the disease. What is the minimum you would ask to be paid?’ Most people set the fee much higher than the price they were willing to pay for the vaccine (ratio of 50:1). The high selling price demonstrates two features of the problem: we are not supposed to sell our health (is viewed as not legitimate, thus people are reluctant to participate) and more importantly: you will be responsible for a potential bad outcome. You will feel more regret in the second case, because you could have done nothing. Another example is the reluctance of parents to expose their child to a danger for a few seconds in return for money. The intense aversion to trading increased risk for a benefit is also found in European laws (precautionary principle: actions that might cause harm are prohibited).

Image  Image  Image  Image

Access: 
Public
This content is used in:

Summary of Thinking, Fast and Slow by Kahneman - 1st edition

Image

This content is also used in .....
Work for WorldSupporter

Image

JoHo can really use your help!  Check out the various student jobs here that match your studies, improve your competencies, strengthen your CV and contribute to a more tolerant world

Working for JoHo as a student in Leyden

Parttime werken voor JoHo

Comments, Compliments & Kudos:

Add new contribution

CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Image CAPTCHA
Enter the characters shown in the image.

Image

Check how to use summaries on WorldSupporter.org

Online access to all summaries, study notes en practice exams

How and why would you use WorldSupporter.org for your summaries and study assistance?

  • For free use of many of the summaries and study aids provided or collected by your fellow students.
  • For free use of many of the lecture and study group notes, exam questions and practice questions.
  • For use of all exclusive summaries and study assistance for those who are member with JoHo WorldSupporter with online access
  • For compiling your own materials and contributions with relevant study help
  • For sharing and finding relevant and interesting summaries, documents, notes, blogs, tips, videos, discussions, activities, recipes, side jobs and more.

Using and finding summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter

There are several ways to navigate the large amount of summaries, study notes en practice exams on JoHo WorldSupporter.

  1. Use the menu above every page to go to one of the main starting pages
    • Starting pages: for some fields of study and some university curricula editors have created (start) magazines where customised selections of summaries are put together to smoothen navigation. When you have found a magazine of your likings, add that page to your favorites so you can easily go to that starting point directly from your profile during future visits. Below you will find some start magazines per field of study
  2. Use the topics and taxonomy terms
    • The topics and taxonomy of the study and working fields gives you insight in the amount of summaries that are tagged by authors on specific subjects. This type of navigation can help find summaries that you could have missed when just using the search tools. Tags are organised per field of study and per study institution. Note: not all content is tagged thoroughly, so when this approach doesn't give the results you were looking for, please check the search tool as back up
  3. Check or follow your (study) organizations:
    • by checking or using your study organizations you are likely to discover all relevant study materials.
    • this option is only available trough partner organizations
  4. Check or follow authors or other WorldSupporters
    • by following individual users, authors  you are likely to discover more relevant study materials.
  5. Use the Search tools
    • 'Quick & Easy'- not very elegant but the fastest way to find a specific summary of a book or study assistance with a specific course or subject.
    • The search tool is also available at the bottom of most pages

Do you want to share your summaries with JoHo WorldSupporter and its visitors?

Quicklinks to fields of study for summaries and study assistance

Field of study

Statistics
1269